Arapaho language
The Arapaho language is one of the Plains Algonquian languages, closely related to Gros Ventre and other Arapahoan languages. It is spoken by the Arapaho of Wyoming and Oklahoma. Speakers of Arapaho primarily live on the Wind River Indian Reservation in Wyoming, though some have affiliation with the Cheyenne living in western Oklahoma.
Classification
Arapaho is an Algonquian language of the Algic family.History
By the 1850s, Arapaho bands formed two tribes: the Northern Arapaho and Southern Arapaho. Since 1878 the Northern Arapaho have lived with the Eastern Shoshone on the Wind River Reservation in Wyoming and are federally recognized as the Arapahoe Tribe of the Wind River Reservation. The Southern Arapaho live with the Southern Cheyenne in Oklahoma. Together their members are enrolled as the federally recognized Cheyenne and Arapaho Tribes.After World War 2, the Northern Arapaho tribe tended to use English, not Arapaho, when raising their children. However, Arapaho speakers within the tribe still primarily speak Arapaho amongst each other. The Northern Arapaho additionally have had relatively less intermingling with other tribes and non-Native Americans compared to the Southern Arapaho who live amongst a predominantly non-Native American population.
Current status
The exact number of Arapaho speakers is not precisely known; however it has been estimated that the language currently retains between 250 and 1,000 active users. Arapaho has limited development outside of the home; however, it is used in some films and the Bible was translated into the language in 1903. According to one source, under 300 people over the age of 50 speak the language in Wyoming, and in Oklahoma the language is used by "only a handful of people... all near eighty or older". As of 1996, there were approximately 1,000 speakers among the Northern Arapaho. As of 2008, the authors of a newly published grammar estimated that there were slightly over 250 fluent speakers, plus "quite a few near-fluent passive understanders". In 2008, it was reported that a school had been opened to teach the language to children. Arapaho language camps were held in Summer 2015 at Wind River Tribal College and in St. Stephens, Wyoming. Currently, the language may be acquired by children, for a population estimate as recent as 2007 lists an increase to 1,000 speakers and notes that the language is in use in schools, bilingual education efforts begun on Wind River Reservation in the 1980s and the Arapaho Language Lodge, a successful immersion program, was established in 1993. "The Arapaho Project" is an effort made by the Arapaho people to promote and restore their traditional language and culture. Despite hope for the language, its relatively few active users and the fact that it has seen recent population decreases render Arapaho an endangered language. Ethnologue deems it "threatened," meaning that some children are learning it but it is threatened by other languages and it may be losing speakers.Dialects
Besawunena, only attested from a wordlist collected by Kroeber, differs only slightly from Arapaho, though a few of its sound changes resemble those seen in Gros Ventre. It had speakers among the Northern Arapaho as recently as the late 1920s.Phonology
Among the sound changes in the evolution from Proto-Algonquian to Arapaho are the loss of Proto-Algonquian *k, followed by *p becoming either or ; the two Proto-Algonquian semivowels merging to either or ; the change from *s to in word-initial position, and *m becoming or depending on the following vowel. Arapaho is unusual among Algonquian languages in retaining the contrast between the reconstructed phonemes *r and *θ. These and other changes serve to give Arapaho a phonological system very divergent from that of Proto-Algonquian and other Algonquian languages, and even from languages spoken in the adjacent Great Basin. Some examples comparing Arapaho words with their cognates in Proto-Algonquian can illustrate this:Proto-Algonquian | Arapaho | Translation |
*erenyiwa | 'man' | |
*wa•poswa | 'hare' | |
*nepyi | 'water' | |
*weθkweni | ' liver' | |
*mexka•či | 'leg' | |
*si•pi•wi | 'river' | |
*sakime•wa | 'mosquito' > 'fly' | |
*akweHmi | 'blanket, robe' | |
*ka•ka•kiwa | 'raven' > 'crow' | |
*aθemwa | 'dog' |
Vowels
At the level of pronunciation, Arapaho words cannot begin with a vowel, so where the underlying form of a word begins with a vowel, a prothetic is added.Arapaho has a series of four short vowels and four long vowels . The difference in length is phonemically distinctive: compare hísiʼ, "tick" with híísiʼ, "day", and hócoo, "steak" with hóócoo, "devil". and are mostly in complementary distribution, as, with very few exceptions, the former does not occur after velar consonants, and the latter only occurs after them. does have some exceptions as in the free variants kokíy ~ kokúy, "gun"; kookiyón ~ kookuyón, "for no reason"; and bííʼoxíyoo ~ bííʼoxúyoo, "Found in the Grass". There is only one minimal pair to illustrate the contrast in distribution: núhuʼ, "this" versus níhiʼ-, "X was done with Y", in which níhiʼ- only occurs in bound form.
Remarkably, unlike more than 98% of the world's languages, Arapaho has no low vowels, such as.
In addition, there are four diphthongs,, and several triphthongs, as well as extended sequences of vowels such as with stress on either the first or the last vowel in the combination.
Front | Back | |
High | ||
Mid |
Consonants
The consonant inventory of Arapaho is given in the table below. When writing Arapaho, is normally transcribed as, as, as, and as.Labial | Dental | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glottal | |
Nasal | ||||||
Stop | ||||||
Fricative | ||||||
Approximant |
Allophony
The phoneme /b/ has a voiceless allophone that occurs before other consonants or at the end of a word. The plosives, /k/, and /t/ are pronounced without aspiration in most environments, but are aspirated before other consonants or at the end of a word, or when preceding a syllable-final sequence of short vowel + /h/. In this same environment /b/ is aspirated and devoiced. For example, the grammatical prefix cih- is pronounced, the grammatical prefix tih- is pronounced, and the word héétbihʼínkúútiinoo, "I will turn out the lights" is hetihʼínkúútiinoo.Syllable Structures
Syllables tend to have the structure CV, where the final consonant, if it is present, is either a single consonant, or /hC/. In general, consonant clusters in Arapaho can only be two consonants long. Consonant clusters do not occur word initially, and /hC/ is the only that occurs word finally. The only consonant cluster that is "base generated" is /hC/. At the "surface", other clusters arise by phonological processes including vowel syncope, or by juxtaposition of morphemes.Vowel-initial, onset-less syllables, however, can occur due to partitioning of vowel clusters. An example of partitioning a cluster of 3 identical vowels into syllables is ní.ii.non, "tepee." The vowel cluster is not always split into short vowel followed by long vowel; the location of the partition depends on Arapaho's complex pitch accent system. For example, another word with a sequence of 3 vowels, but with a different partitioning of vowels into syllables is hóo.ó. "bed."
However, sometimes the vowel cluster does not divide and the whole cluster becomes the nucleus of the syllable. One example is hi.héio, "his/her aunt."
Prosody
Arapaho is a pitch accent language. There are two phonemic tones: high or "normal". The contrast can be illustrated with the pair hónoosóóʼ, "it is fancy" and honoosóóʼ, "it is raining." Long vowels and vowel sequences can carry a contour tone from high to low, as in hou3íne-, "to hang" versus hóu3íne-, "to float". Although tonal contrasts are distinctive in Arapaho, minimal pairs such as those listed above are rare.Morphology
Arapaho is polysynthetic; verbs in particular take multiple morphemes.Inflectional
Nouns
Animacy
Nouns in Arapaho come in two classes: animate and inanimate. Which category a noun belongs to is part of the lexicon. Being animate does not necessitate “aliveness” : doors, planets, ghosts, etc. are considered animate. Some nouns can also be both animate and inanimate, but in these situations, the animate version is more “active”.Obviation
Animate nouns can be made obviative or proximate.When the underlying noun is consonant final, two general patterns can occur. One pattern occurs for the class of nouns that have /ii/ or /uu/ as their plural marker. These nouns reuse the plural marker to mark obviative singular and both obviative and proximate plural. For example, /iwoxuuh/, a stem meaning 'elk,' is hiwóxuu in the proximate singular, but becomes hiwóxuuh-uu in obviative singular, proximate plural, and obviative plural.
The other pattern occurs for most other consonant final noun stems and is summarized in the table below. C denotes the final consonant and the bracketed denotes either consonant mutation of C or deletion of some number of stem-final phonemes. /siisiik/ is 'duck'.
Proximate | Obviative | |
Singular | C | |
Plural | C-o' | C-o |
For vowel-final stems, the general pattern is a variation of the first consonant final pattern. Namely, a single marker is used to mark all plural forms and the obviative singular form. For example, /ote/, a stem meaning 'sheep, bighorn sheep,' becomes hóte-’ in proximate singular but hóte-ii in both plural forms and the obviative singular.
Verbs
Verb categorization
Verbs are divided into classes depending on the transitivity and animacy of their argument. Transitivity of a verb affects how many arguments are affixed to the verb. Notice in the examples below the usage of the transitive form requires the addition of INAN, the inanimate marker for the object. Verbal inflection also depends on "orders" like imperative.Intransitive, Animate Subject
Transitive, Inanimate Object
Initial Change
Initial change can mark tense and aspect under affirmative and conjunct orders. Differing phonological changes occur depending on the first vowel of the stem. If the vowel is short, it is lengthened. For example, be’éé- 'to be red' becomes bee’éé’ 'it is red'. Otherwise, an infix is placed before the first vowel. The infix is either /en/ or /on/ and is determined based on harmony with the long vowel. For example, hoowúsee- becomes honoowúseenoo.If the first vowel is short and is followed by an /h/, some speakers treat the /h/ as a vowel and use the infix /en/ or /on/ to mark initial change. Other speakers treat the /h/ as a consonant and perform the vowel lengthening process instead.
An irregular form of initial change affects some vowel-initial preverbs by appending an /n/ before the first vowel, rather than the ordinary /h/ that would be prepended to avoid a vowel-initial word. For example, the imperfective /ii/ morpheme becomes nii- instead of the expected hii- when prefixing verbs that would undergo initial change.
Agreement
In sentences with an explicit noun phrase, separate from the verb, the verb agrees with the noun in terms of animacy, number, and whether the noun is proximate or obviative. The grammatical category, including person, of the noun also needs to agree with the verb. Note that the categories of subject and object do not affect agreement inflection. As an example of animacy agreement, the intransitive verb for "to fall" has a form that takes an inanimate subject, nihtéésceníse’ and a form that takes an animate subject, nihtéés'If a verb has a single noun argument that is composed of two different types of noun, most speakers default to the obviative and inanimate forms to refer to the composite noun argument in case of conflict. This can be seen in the example below where 'walk' takes an argument that is composed of a composite proximate and obviative noun. Both nouns are animate, but there is conflict regarding proximate or obviative. The verb thus defaults to the obviative plural.
Derivational
Nouns
Arapaho has a number of derivational affixes and processes. Some operate on nouns to form verb-like clauses. For example, the morpheme /tohúút/ can prefix a noun to ask 'what kind ofVerbs
Derivational morphology on verbs can be grouped into abstract and concrete. Abstract morphemes mark transitivity and the animacy of subject/object for the verb. For example, the basic root /be'/ "red" can be marked with abstract morphemes as follows- /be'-ee/ "to be red," intransitive and takes an inanimate subject
- /be'-eihi/ "to be red," intransitive and takes an animate subject
- patients and undergoers; attach particularly common nouns after a verb with a transitive meaning to give it an object, resulting in an intransitive verb
- *Example: to add /-oox-/ "wood" as an object, transform it to /-ooxu-/ before appending to verb such as /no’ooxu-/ and /cowooxu-/
- topics concerning nouns that lack volition; examples include body parts, weather and nature, sensations
- *Example: the underlying noun be-sonon becomes /-isono-/ before attaching to a verb such as /enisono-/
- methods of achieving action; examples include tools, means of transportation, non-manmade forces such as wind
- *Example: /-see/ can be added to create an AI verb such as no’usee- and oowusee-
Reduplication
There are multiple usages of reduplication in Arapaho including pluralizing implied, secondary, and inanimate objects of transitive verbs; indicating repeating and habitual action, and intensifying. One example of marking repeating action is as follows
There can be multiple reduplications in compound words, where each reduplication can have an independent effect. Some verbs appear to be only in a reduplicated form; these verbs tend to describe repeating, iterative action.
Syntax
Arapaho has no canonical word order. Some sentences/clauses consist of only the verb like below.Single Noun Phrase
When a sentence contains a verb and a single noun phrase, the noun phrase can either precede or follow the verb. Preposing the noun phrase, however, gives it more importance and salience. Some instances where noun phrases are preposed include introducing a new referent and creating contrast.V-NP order
NP-V order
Two Noun Phrases
For a basic sentence with a single verb that takes two noun phrases as arguments, all orderings are possible, but having the verb final is less common.Noun Phrase Hierarchy
A hierarchy exists in determining which noun phrase goes in which position. In the listing below, the first in the pair is treated as "higher" in the hierarchy and tends therefore to be the leftmost NP.- subject
- proximate
- actor
- marked object
- animate
Verb-subject- object order
Saliency
Saliency determines whether a noun phrase can precede its corresponding verb. Noun phrases are deemed salient if they are referring to something new, something that is being reintroduced, something contrastive, or something that is being emphasized.Preposed NP
Both NPs preceding Verb
Syntax of Noun/Verb Phrases
Modifying Nouns
Generally, noun modifiers occur before the noun. These modifiers additionally tend to occur in a particular order relative each other. For example, in the example below, note that the presentative 'here is' occurs before the demonstrative 'this'Particles and Verb Stems
Some particles are more closely linked to verbs; these particles generally precede the verb and are often neighboring the corresponding verb.Particle expressing potential
Particle expressing recent past
Adverbials
Adverbials are a type of particle. Unlike other particles in Arapaho, however, they are not a closed class and are instead derived from or composed of other morphemes. One purpose of adverbial construction is to emphasize a morpheme by extracting it from a verb and having it stand alone. Another purpose is to convey meaning outside of what can normally be attached to a verb.Adverbials are constructed by appending /iihi'/ to the end of the root.
A common usage of adverbials is to modify verbs. Adverbials can also act like prepositions and modify noun phrases; such adverbials can occur before or after the noun phrase and are thus exceptions to the rule that nominal modifiers prepose the noun.