Average treatment effect
The average treatment effect is a measure used to compare treatments in randomized experiments, evaluation of policy interventions, and medical trials. The ATE measures the difference in mean outcomes between units assigned to the treatment and units assigned to the control. In a randomized trial, the average treatment effect can be estimated from a sample using a comparison in mean outcomes for treated and untreated units. However, the ATE is generally understood as a causal parameter that a researcher desires to know, defined without reference to the study design or estimation procedure. Both observational studies and experimental study designs with random assignment may enable one to estimate an ATE in a variety of ways.
General definition
Originating from early statistical analysis in the fields of agriculture and medicine, the term "treatment" is now applied, more generally, to other fields of natural and social science, especially psychology, political science, and economics such as, for example, the evaluation of the impact of public policies. The nature of a treatment or outcome is relatively unimportant in the estimation of the ATE—that is to say, calculation of the ATE requires that a treatment be applied to some units and not others, but the nature of that treatment is irrelevant to the definition and estimation of the ATE.The expression "treatment effect" refers to the causal effect of a given treatment or intervention on an outcome variable of interest. In the Neyman-Rubin "Potential Outcomes Framework" of causality a treatment effect is defined for each individual unit in terms of two "potential outcomes." Each unit has one outcome that would manifest if the unit were exposed to the treatment and another outcome that would manifest if the unit were exposed to the control. The "treatment effect" is the difference between these two potential outcomes. However, this individual-level treatment effect is unobservable because individual units can only receive the treatment or the control, but not both. Random assignment to treatment ensures that units assigned to the treatment and units assigned to the control are identical. Indeed, units in both groups have identical distributions of covariates and potential outcomes. Thus the average outcome among the treatment units serves as a counterfactual for the average outcome among the control units. The differences between these two averages is the ATE, which is an estimate of the central tendency of the distribution of unobservable individual-level treatment effects. If a sample is randomly constituted from a population, the ATE from the sample is also an estimate of the population ATE.
While an experiment ensures, in expectation, that potential outcomes are equivalently distributed in the treatment and control groups, this is not the case in an observational study. In an observational study, units are not assigned to treatment and control randomly, so their assignment to treatment may depend on unobserved or unobservable factors. Observed factors can be statistically controlled, but any estimate of the ATE could be confounded by unobservable factors that influenced which units received the treatment versus the control.
Formal definition
In order to define formally the ATE, we define two potential outcomes : is the value of the outcome variable for individual if they are not treated, is the value of the outcome variable for individual ifthey are treated. For example, is the health status of the individual if they are not administered the drug under study and is the health status if they are administered the drug.
The treatment effect for individual is given by. In the general case, there is no reason to expect this effect to be constant across individuals. The average treatment effect is given by
where the summation occurs over all individuals in the population.
If we could observe, for each individual, and among a large representative sample of the population, we could estimate the ATE simply by taking the average value of across the sample.
The problem is that we can not observe both and for each individual. For example, in the drug example, we can only observe for individuals who have received the drug and for those who did not receive it; we do not observe for treated individuals and for untreated ones. This fact is the main problem faced by scientists in the evaluation of treatment effects and has triggered a large body of estimation techniques.
Estimation
Depending on the data and its underlying circumstances, many methods can be used to estimate the ATE. The most common ones are- Natural experiment and the similar quasi-experiment,
- Difference in differences or its short version: diff-in-diffs,
- the Regression discontinuity design method,
- matching method,
- methods based on the theory of local IVs
where y is the response variable and measures the effects of the policy change on the population.
The difference in differences equation would be
where T is the treatment group and C is the control group. In this case the measures the effects of the treatment on the average outcome and is the average treatment effect.
From the diffs-in-diffs example we can see the main problems of estimating treatment effects. As we can not observe the same individual as treated and non-treated at the same time, we have to come up with a measure of counterfactuals to estimate the average treatment effect.
An example
Consider an example where all units are unemployed individuals, and some experience a policy intervention, while others do not. The causal effect of interest is the impact a job search monitoring policy has on the length of an unemployment spell: On average, how much shorter would one's unemployment be if they experienced the intervention? The ATE, in this case, is the difference in expected values of the treatment and control groups' length of unemployment.A positive ATE, in this example, would suggest that the job policy increased the length of unemployment. A negative ATE would suggest that the job policy decreased the length of unemployment. An ATE estimate equal to zero would suggest that there was no advantage or disadvantage to providing the treatment in terms of the length of unemployment. Determining whether an ATE estimate is distinguishable from zero requires statistical inference.
Because the ATE is an estimate of the average effect of the treatment, a positive or negative ATE does not indicate that any particular individual would benefit or be harmed by the treatment. Thus the average treatment effect neglects the distribution of the treatment effect. Some parts of the population might be worse off with the treatment even if the mean effect is positive.
Heterogenous treatment effects
Some researchers call a treatment effect "heterogenous" if it affects different individuals differently. For example, perhaps the above treatment of a job search monitoring policy affected men and women differently, or people who live in different states differently. One way to look for heterogeneous treatment effects is to divide the study data into subgroups, and see if the average treatment effects are different by subgroup. A challenge with this approach is that each subgroup may have substantially less data than the study as a whole, so if the study has been powered to detect the main effects without subgroup analysis, there may not be enough data to properly judge the effects on subgroups.There is some work on detecting heterogenous treatment effects using random forests.