List of cognitive biases


es are systematic patterns of deviation from norm and/or rationality in judgment. They are often studied in psychology and behavioral economics.
Although the reality of most of these biases is confirmed by reproducible research, there are often controversies about how to classify these biases or how to explain them. Gerd Gigerenzer has criticized the framing of cognitive biases as errors in judgment, and favors interpreting them as arising from rational deviations from logical thought.
Explanations include information-processing rules, called heuristics, that the brain uses to produce decisions or judgments. Biases have a variety of forms and appear as cognitive bias, such as mental noise, or motivational bias, such as when beliefs are distorted by wishful thinking. Both effects can be present at the same time.
There are also controversies over some of these biases as to whether they count as useless or irrational, or whether they result in useful attitudes or behavior. For example, when getting to know others, people tend to ask leading questions which seem biased towards confirming their assumptions about the person. However, this kind of confirmation bias has also been argued to be an example of social skill; a way to establish a connection with the other person.
Although this research overwhelmingly involves human subjects, some findings that demonstrate bias have been found in non-human animals as well. For example, loss aversion has been shown in monkeys and hyperbolic discounting has been observed in rats, pigeons, and monkeys.

Belief, Decision-making and behavioral biases

These biases affect belief formation, reasoning processes, business and economic decisions, and human behavior in general.
NameTypeDescription
Agent detectionFalse priorsThe inclination to presume the purposeful intervention of a sentient or intelligent agent.
Ambiguity effectProspect theoryThe tendency to avoid options for which the probability of a favorable outcome is unknown.
Anchoring or focalismAnchoring biasThe tendency to rely too heavily, or "anchor", on one trait or piece of information when making decisions.
Anthropocentric thinkingAvailability biasThe tendency to use human analogies as a basis for reasoning about other, less familiar, biological phenomena.
Anthropomorphism or personificationAvailability biasThe tendency to characterize animals, objects, and abstract concepts as possessing human-like traits, emotions, and intentions. The opposite bias, of not attributing feelings or thoughts to another person, is dehumanised perception, a type of objectification.
Attentional biasAvailability biasThe tendency of perception to be affected by recurring thoughts.
Attribute substitutionOccurs when a judgment has to be made that is computationally complex, and instead a more easily calculated heuristic attribute is substituted. This substitution is thought of as taking place in the automatic intuitive judgment system, rather than the more self-aware reflective system.
Automation biasFalse priorsThe tendency to depend excessively on automated systems which can lead to erroneous automated information overriding correct decisions.
Availability heuristicAvailability biasThe tendency to overestimate the likelihood of events with greater "availability" in memory, which can be influenced by how recent the memories are or how unusual or emotionally charged they may be.
Backfire effectConfirmation biasThe reaction to disconfirming evidence by strengthening one's previous beliefs. Note: the existence of this bias as a widespread phenomenon has been disputed in empirical studies
Base rate fallacy or Base rate neglectExtension neglectThe tendency to ignore general information and focus on information only pertaining to the specific case, even when the general information is more important.
Belief biasTruthinessAn effect where someone's evaluation of the logical strength of an argument is biased by the believability of the conclusion.
Berkson's paradoxLogical fallacyThe tendency to misinterpret statistical experiments involving conditional probabilities.
Clustering illusionApopheniaThe tendency to overestimate the importance of small runs, streaks, or clusters in large samples of random data.
Compassion fadeExtension neglectThe predisposition to behave more compassionately towards a small number of identifiable victims than to a large number of anonymous ones.
Confirmation biasConfirmation biasThe tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions.
Congruence biasConfirmation biasThe tendency to test hypotheses exclusively through direct testing, instead of testing possible alternative hypotheses.
Conjunction fallacyExtension neglectThe tendency to assume that specific conditions are more probable than a more general version of those same conditions. For example, subjects in one experiment perceived the probability of a woman being both a bank teller and a feminist as more likely than the probability of her being a bank teller.
Conservatism bias Anchoring biasThe tendency to revise one's belief insufficiently when presented with new evidence.
Continued influence effectConfirmation biasThe tendency to believe previously learned misinformation even after it has been corrected. Misinformation can still influence inferences one generates after a correction has occurred. cf. Backfire effect
Contrast effectFraming effectThe enhancement or reduction of a certain stimulus' perception when compared with a recently observed, contrasting object.
Curse of knowledgeWhen better-informed people find it extremely difficult to think about problems from the perspective of lesser-informed people.
DeclinismThe predisposition to view the past favorably and future negatively.
Decoy effectFraming effectPreferences for either option A or B change in favor of option B when option C is presented, which is completely dominated by option B and partially dominated by option A.
Default effectFraming effectWhen given a choice between several options, the tendency to favor the default one.
Denomination effectFraming effectThe tendency to spend more money when it is denominated in small amounts rather than large amounts.
Disposition effectProspect theoryThe tendency to sell an asset that has accumulated in value and resist selling an asset that has declined in value.
Distinction biasFraming effectThe tendency to view two options as more dissimilar when evaluating them simultaneously than when evaluating them separately.
Dread aversionProspect theoryJust as losses yield double the emotional impact of gains, dread yields double the emotional impact of savouring.
Dunning–Kruger effectThe tendency for unskilled individuals to overestimate their own ability and the tendency for experts to underestimate their own ability.
Duration neglectExtension neglectThe neglect of the duration of an episode in determining its value.
Empathy gapThe tendency to underestimate the influence or strength of feelings, in either oneself or others.
End-of-history illusionThe age-independent belief that one will change less in the future than one has in the past.
Endowment effectProspect theoryThe tendency for people to demand much more to give up an object than they would be willing to pay to acquire it.
The tendency to expect or predict more extreme outcomes than those outcomes that actually happen.
Experimenter's or expectation biasConfirmation biasThe tendency for experimenters to believe, certify, and publish data that agree with their expectations for the outcome of an experiment, and to disbelieve, discard, or downgrade the corresponding weightings for data that appear to conflict with those expectations.
Forer effect or Barnum effectEgocentric biasThe observation that individuals will give high accuracy ratings to descriptions of their personality that supposedly are tailored specifically for them, but are in fact vague and general enough to apply to a wide range of people. This effect can provide a partial explanation for the widespread acceptance of some beliefs and practices, such as astrology, fortune telling, graphology, and some types of personality tests.
Form function attribution biasIn human–robot interaction, the tendency of people to make systematic errors when interacting with a robot. People may base their expectations and perceptions of a robot on its appearance and attribute functions which do not necessarily mirror the true functions of the robot.
Framing effectFraming effectDrawing different conclusions from the same information, depending on how that information is presented.
Frequency illusion or Baader–Meinhof phenomenonAvailability biasThe frequency illusion is that once something has been noticed then every instance of that thing is noticed, leading to the belief it has a high frequency of occurrence. The Baader–Meinhof phenomenon is the illusion where something that has recently come to one's attention suddenly seems to appear with improbable frequency shortly afterwards. The Baader–Meinhof phenomenon is sometimes conflated with frequency illusion and the recency illusion. It was named after an incidence of frequency illusion in which the Baader–Meinhof Group was mentioned.
Functional fixednessAnchoring biasLimits a person to using an object only in the way it is traditionally used.
Gambler's fallacyLogical fallacyThe tendency to think that future probabilities are altered by past events, when in reality they are unchanged. The fallacy arises from an erroneous conceptualization of the law of large numbers. For example, "I've flipped heads with this coin five times consecutively, so the chance of tails coming out on the sixth flip is much greater than heads."
Gender biasFalse priorsA widely held set of implicit biases that discriminate against a gender. For example, the assumption that women are less suited to jobs requiring high intellectual ability. Or the assumption that people or animals are male in the absence of any indicators of gender.
Hard–easy effectThe tendency to overestimate one's ability to accomplish hard tasks, and underestimate one's ability to accomplish easy tasks
Hindsight biasSometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" effect, the tendency to see past events as being predictable at the time those events happened.
Hot-hand fallacyLogical fallacyThe "hot-hand fallacy" is the belief that a person who has experienced success with a random event has a greater chance of further success in additional attempts.
Hyperbolic discountingExtension neglectDiscounting is the tendency for people to have a stronger preference for more immediate payoffs relative to later payoffs. Hyperbolic discounting leads to choices that are inconsistent over time – people make choices today that their future selves would prefer not to have made, despite using the same reasoning. Also known as current moment bias, present-bias, and related to Dynamic inconsistency. A good example of this: a study showed that when making food choices for the coming week, 74% of participants chose fruit, whereas when the food choice was for the current day, 70% chose chocolate.
IKEA effectThe tendency for people to place a disproportionately high value on objects that they partially assembled themselves, such as furniture from IKEA, regardless of the quality of the end product.
Illicit transferenceLogical fallacyOccurs when a term in the distributive and collective sense are treated as equivalent. The two variants of this fallacy are the fallacy of composition and the fallacy of division.
Illusion of controlEgocentric biasThe tendency to overestimate one's degree of influence over other external events.
Illusion of validityEgocentric biasBelieving that one's judgments are accurate, especially when available information is consistent or inter-correlated.
Illusory correlationApopheniaInaccurately perceiving a relationship between two unrelated events.
Illusory truth effectTruthinessA tendency to believe that a statement is true if it is easier to process, or if it has been stated multiple times, regardless of its actual veracity. These are specific cases of truthiness.
Impact biasThe tendency to overestimate the length or the intensity of the impact of future feeling states.
Implicit associationAvailability biasThe speed with which people can match words depends on how closely they are associated.
Information biasThe tendency to seek information even when it cannot affect action.
Insensitivity to sample sizeExtension neglectThe tendency to under-expect variation in small samples.
Interoceptive biasThe tendency for sensory input about the body itself to affect one's judgement about external, unrelated circumstances.
Irrational escalation or Escalation of commitmentLogical fallacyThe phenomenon where people justify increased investment in a decision, based on the cumulative prior investment, despite new evidence suggesting that the decision was probably wrong. Also known as the sunk cost fallacy.
Law of the instrumentAnchoring biasAn over-reliance on a familiar tool or methods, ignoring or under-valuing alternative approaches. "If all you have is a hammer, everything looks like a nail."
Less-is-better effectExtension neglectThe tendency to prefer a smaller set to a larger set judged separately, but not jointly.
Loss aversionProspect theoryThe perceived disutility of giving up an object is greater than the utility associated with acquiring it..
Mere exposure effectFamiliarity principleThe tendency to express undue liking for things merely because of familiarity with them.
Money illusionThe tendency to concentrate on the nominal value of money rather than its value in terms of purchasing power.
Moral credential effectOccurs when someone who does something good gives themselves permission to be less good in the future.
Neglect of probabilityExtension neglectThe tendency to completely disregard probability when making a decision under uncertainty.
Non-adaptive choice switchingAfter experiencing a bad outcome with a decision problem, the tendency to avoid the choice previously made when faced with the same decision problem again, even though the choice was optimal. Also known as "once bitten, twice shy" or "hot stove effect".
Normalcy biasCognitive dissonanceThe refusal to plan for, or react to, a disaster which has never happened before.
Observer-expectancy effectConfirmation biasWhen a researcher expects a given result and therefore unconsciously manipulates an experiment or misinterprets data in order to find it.
Omission biasThe tendency to judge harmful actions as worse, or less moral, than equally harmful inactions.
Optimism biasThe tendency to be over-optimistic, underestimating greatly the probability of undesirable outcomes and overestimating favorable and pleasing outcomes.
Ostrich effectIgnoring an obvious situation.
Outcome biasThe tendency to judge a decision by its eventual outcome instead of based on the quality of the decision at the time it was made.
Overconfidence effectEgocentric biasExcessive confidence in one's own answers to questions. For example, for certain types of questions, answers that people rate as "99% certain" turn out to be wrong 40% of the time.
PareidoliaApopheniaA vague and random stimulus is perceived as significant, e.g., seeing images of animals or faces in clouds, the man in the moon, and hearing non-existent hidden messages on records played in reverse.
Pessimism biasThe tendency for some people, especially those suffering from depression, to overestimate the likelihood of negative things happening to them.
Plan continuation biasLogical fallacyFailure to recognize that the original plan of action is no longer appropriate for a changing situation or for a situation that is different than anticipated.
Planning fallacyEgocentric biasThe tendency to underestimate one's own task-completion times.
Present biasThe tendency of people to give stronger weight to payoffs that are closer to the present time when considering trade-offs between two future moments.
Plant blindnessThe tendency to ignore plants in their environment and a failure to recognize and appreciate the utility of plants to life on earth.
Pro-innovation biasThe tendency to have an excessive optimism towards an invention or innovation's usefulness throughout society, while often failing to identify its limitations and weaknesses.
Projection biasThe tendency to overestimate how much our future selves share one's current preferences, thoughts and values, thus leading to sub-optimal choices.
Proportionality BiasOur innate tendency to assume that big events have big causes, may also explain our tendency to accept conspiracies.
Pseudocertainty effectProspect theoryThe tendency to make risk-averse choices if the expected outcome is positive, but make risk-seeking choices to avoid negative outcomes.
Recency illusionThe illusion that a phenomenon one has noticed only recently is itself recent. Often used to refer to linguistic phenomena; the illusion that a word or language usage that one has noticed only recently is an innovation when it is in fact long-established.
Systematic BiasJudgement that arises when targets of differentiating judgement become subject to effects of regression that are not equivalent.
Restraint biasEgocentric biasThe tendency to overestimate one's ability to show restraint in the face of temptation.
Rhyme as reason effectTruthinessRhyming statements are perceived as more truthful. A famous example being used in the O.J Simpson trial with the defense's use of the phrase "If the gloves don't fit, then you must acquit."
Risk compensation / Peltzman effectThe tendency to take greater risks when perceived safety increases.
Salience biasAvailability biasThe tendency to focus on items that are more prominent or emotionally striking and ignore those that are unremarkable, even though this difference is often irrelevant by objective standards.
Scope neglect or scope insensitivityExtension neglectThe tendency to be insensitive to the size of a problem when evaluating it. For example, being willing to pay as much to save 2,000 children or 20,000 children
Selection biasAvailability biasThe tendency to notice something more when something causes us to be more aware of it, such as when we buy a car, we tend to notice similar cars more often than we did before. They are not suddenly more common – we just are noticing them more. Also called the Observational Selection Bias.
Selective perceptionConfirmation biasThe tendency for expectations to affect perception.
Semmelweis reflexConfirmation biasThe tendency to reject new evidence that contradicts a paradigm.
Status quo biasProspect theoryThe tendency to like things to stay relatively the same.
StereotypingFalse priorsExpecting a member of a group to have certain characteristics without having actual information about that individual.
Subadditivity effectLogical fallacyThe tendency to judge probability of the whole to be less than the probabilities of the parts.
Subjective validationTruthinessPerception that something is true if a subject's belief demands it to be true. Also assigns perceived connections between coincidences.
SurrogationLosing sight of the strategic construct that a measure is intended to represent, and subsequently acting as though the measure is the construct of interest.
Survivorship biasAvailability biasConcentrating on the people or things that "survived" some process and inadvertently overlooking those that didn't because of their lack of visibility.
System justificationProspect theoryThe tendency to defend and bolster the status quo. Existing social, economic, and political arrangements tend to be preferred, and alternatives disparaged, sometimes even at the expense of individual and collective self-interest.
Time-saving biasLogical fallacyUnderestimations of the time that could be saved when increasing from a relatively low speed and overestimations of the time that could be saved when increasing from a relatively high speed.
Parkinson's law of trivialityThe tendency to give disproportionate weight to trivial issues. Also known as bikeshedding, this bias explains why an organization may avoid specialized or complex subjects, such as the design of a nuclear reactor, and instead focus on something easy to grasp or rewarding to the average participant, such as the design of an adjacent bike shed.
The standard suggested amount of consumption is perceived to be appropriate, and a person would consume it all even if it is too much for this particular person.
Weber–Fechner lawDifficulty in comparing small differences in large quantities.
Well travelled road effectAvailability biasUnderestimation of the duration taken to traverse oft-traveled routes and overestimation of the duration taken to traverse less familiar routes.
Women are wonderful effectA tendency to associate more positive attributes with women than with men.
Zero-risk biasExtension neglectPreference for reducing a small risk to zero over a greater reduction in a larger risk.
Zero-sum biasLogical fallacyA bias whereby a situation is incorrectly perceived to be like a zero-sum game.

Social biases

Memory errors and biases

In psychology and cognitive science, a memory bias is a cognitive bias that either enhances or impairs the recall of a memory, or that alters the content of a reported memory. There are many types of memory bias, including:
NameDescription
Bizarreness effectBizarre material is better remembered than common material.
Choice-supportive biasThe tendency to remember one's choices as better than they actually were.
or Regressive biasTendency to remember high values and high likelihoods/probabilities/frequencies as lower than they actually were and low ones as higher than they actually were. Based on the evidence, memories are not extreme enough.
Incorrectly remembering one's past attitudes and behaviour as resembling present attitudes and behaviour.
Context effectThat cognition and memory are dependent on context, such that out-of-context memories are more difficult to retrieve than in-context memories.
Cross-race effectThe tendency for people of one race to have difficulty identifying members of a race other than their own.
CryptomnesiaA form of misattribution where a memory is mistaken for imagination, because there is no subjective experience of it being a memory.
Egocentric biasRecalling the past in a self-serving manner, e.g., remembering one's exam grades as being better than they were, or remembering a caught fish as bigger than it really was.
Fading affect biasA bias in which the emotion associated with unpleasant memories fades more quickly than the emotion associated with positive events.
False memoryA form of misattribution where imagination is mistaken for a memory.
Generation effect That self-generated information is remembered best. For instance, people are better able to recall memories of statements that they have generated than similar statements generated by others.
Google effectThe tendency to forget information that can be found readily online by using Internet search engines.
That humorous items are more easily remembered than non-humorous ones, which might be explained by the distinctiveness of humor, the increased cognitive processing time to understand the humor, or the emotional arousal caused by the humor.
Lag effectThe phenomenon whereby learning is greater when studying is spread out over time, as opposed to studying the same amount of time in a single session. See also spacing effect.
Leveling and sharpeningMemory distortions introduced by the loss of details in a recollection over time, often concurrent with sharpening or selective recollection of certain details that take on exaggerated significance in relation to the details or aspects of the experience lost through leveling. Both biases may be reinforced over time, and by repeated recollection or re-telling of a memory.
Levels-of-processing effectThat different methods of encoding information into memory have different levels of effectiveness.
A smaller percentage of items are remembered in a longer list, but as the length of the list increases, the absolute number of items remembered increases as well. For example, consider a list of 30 items and a list of 100 items. An individual may remember 15 items from L30, or 50%, whereas the individual may remember 40 items from L100, or 40%. Although the percent of L30 items remembered is greater than the percent of L100, more L100 items are remembered than L30 items.
Misinformation effectMemory becoming less accurate because of interference from post-event information.
Modality effectThat memory recall is higher for the last items of a list when the list items were received via speech than when they were received through writing.
Mood-congruent memory biasThe improved recall of information congruent with one's current mood.
Negativity bias or Negativity effectPsychological phenomenon by which humans have a greater recall of unpleasant memories compared with positive memories..
Next-in-line effectWhen taking turns speaking in a group using a predetermined order people tend to have diminished recall for the words of the person who spoke immediately before them.
Part-list cueing effectThat being shown some items from a list and later retrieving one item causes it to become harder to retrieve the other items.
Peak-end ruleThat people seem to perceive not the sum of an experience but the average of how it was at its peak and how it ended.
Picture superiority effectThe notion that concepts that are learned by viewing pictures are more easily and frequently recalled than are concepts that are learned by viewing their written word form counterparts.
Positivity effect That older adults favor positive over negative information in their memories.
Serial position effectThat items near the end of a sequence are the easiest to recall, followed by the items at the beginning of a sequence; items in the middle are the least likely to be remembered.
That information that takes longer to read and is thought about more is more easily remembered.
Reminiscence bumpThe recalling of more personal events from adolescence and early adulthood than personal events from other lifetime periods.
Self-relevance effectThat memories relating to the self are better recalled than similar information relating to others.
Source confusionConfusing episodic memories with other information, creating distorted memories.
Spacing effectThat information is better recalled if exposure to it is repeated over a long span of time rather than a short one.
Spotlight effectThe tendency to overestimate the amount that other people notice your appearance or behavior.
Memory distorted towards stereotypes.
Diminishment of the recency effect because a sound item is appended to the list that the subject is not required to recall.
SuggestibilityA form of misattribution where ideas suggested by a questioner are mistaken for memory.
TachypsychiaWhen time perceived by the individual either lengthens, making events appear to slow down, or contracts.
Telescoping effectThe tendency to displace recent events backward in time and remote events forward in time, so that recent events appear more remote, and remote events, more recent.
Testing effectThe fact that you more easily remember information you have read by rewriting it instead of rereading it.
Tip of the tongue phenomenonWhen a subject is able to recall parts of an item, or related information, but is frustratingly unable to recall the whole item. This is thought to be an instance of "blocking" where multiple similar memories are being recalled and interfere with each other.
Overestimating the significance of the present. It is related to chronological snobbery with possibly an appeal to novelty logical fallacy being part of the bias.
That the "gist" of what someone has said is better remembered than the verbatim wording. This is because memories are representations, not exact copies.
von Restorff effectThat an item that sticks out is more likely to be remembered than other items.
Zeigarnik effectThat uncompleted or interrupted tasks are remembered better than completed ones.

Footnotes