Music psychology
Music psychology, or the psychology of music, may be regarded as a branch of both psychology and musicology. It aims to explain and understand musical behaviour and experience, including the processes through which music is perceived, created, responded to, and incorporated into everyday life. Modern music psychology is primarily empirical; its knowledge tends to advance on the basis of interpretations of data collected by systematic observation of and interaction with human participants. Music psychology is a field of research with practical relevance for many areas, including music performance, composition, education, criticism, and therapy, as well as investigations of human attitude, skill, performance, intelligence, creativity, and social behavior.
Music psychology can shed light on non-psychological aspects of musicology and musical practice. For example, it contributes to music theory through investigations of the perception and computational modelling of musical structures such as melody, harmony, tonality, rhythm, meter, and form. Research in music history can benefit from systematic study of the history of musical syntax, or from psychological analyses of composers and compositions in relation to perceptual, affective, and social responses to their music.
History
Early history (pre-1860)
The study of sound and musical phenomenon prior to the 19th century was focused primarily on the mathematical modelling of pitch and tone. The earliest recorded experiments date from the 6th century BCE, most notably in the work of Pythagoras and his establishment of the simple string length ratios that formed the consonances of the octave. This view that sound and music could be understood from a purely physical standpoint was echoed by such theorists as Anaxagoras and Boethius. An important early dissenter was Aristoxenus, who foreshadowed modern music psychology in his view that music could only be understood through human perception and its relation to human memory. Despite his views, the majority of musical education through the Middle Ages and Renaissance remained rooted in the Pythagorean tradition, particularly through the quadrivium of astronomy, geometry, arithmetic, and music.Research by Vincenzo Galilei demonstrated that, when string length was held constant, varying its tension, thickness, or composition could alter perceived pitch. From this he argued that simple ratios were not enough to account for musical phenomenon and that a perceptual approach was necessary. He also claimed that the differences between various tuning systems were not perceivable, thus the disputes were unnecessary.
Study of topics including vibration, consonance, the harmonic series, and resonance were furthered through the scientific revolution, including work by Galileo, Kepler, Mersenne, and Descartes. This included further speculation concerning the nature of the sense organs and higher-order processes, particularly by Savart, Helmholtz, and Koenig.
Rise of empirical study (1860–1960)
The latter 19th century saw the development of modern music psychology alongside the emergence of a general empirical psychology, one which passed through similar stages of development. The first was structuralist psychology, led by Wilhelm Wundt, which sought to break down experience into its smallest definable parts. This expanded upon previous centuries of acoustic study, and included Helmholtz developing the resonator to isolate and understand pure and complex tones and their perception, the philosopher Carl Stumpf using church organs and his own musical experience to explore timbre and absolute pitch, and Wundt himself associating the experience of rhythm with kinesthetic tension and relaxation.As structuralism gave way to Gestalt psychology and behaviorism at the turn of the century, music psychology moved beyond the study of isolated tones and elements to the perception of their inter-relationships and human reactions to them, though work languished behind that of visual perception. In Europe Géza Révész and Albert Wellek developed a more complex understanding of musical pitch, and in the US the focus shifted to that of music education and the training and development of musical skill. Carl Seashore led this work, producing his The Measurement of Musical Talents and The Psychology of Musical Talent. Seashore used bespoke equipment and standardized tests to measure how performance deviated from indicated markings and how musical aptitude differed between students.
In 1963 F. Chrysler was the first one to used the term " science of music" when he was working on his "year book for musical" knowledge. European musicology was found in Greek. They were focus on the philosophy, and the concepts of any relations with music. Greek's several theories rose later on to Arab and the Christians Theories. Although their theories survived, they were also corrupted along the way, in the Middle Ages of Europe.
Modern (1960–present)
Music psychology in the second half of the 20th century has expanded to cover a wide array of theoretical and applied areas. From the 1960s the field grew along with cognitive science, including such research areas as music perception, musical development and aptitude, music performance, and affective responses to music.This period has also seen the founding of music psychology-specific journals, societies, conferences, research groups, centers, and degrees, a trend that has brought research toward specific applications for music education, performance, and therapy. While the techniques of cognitive psychology allowed for more objective examinations of musical behavior and experience, the theoretical and technological advancements of neuroscience have greatly shaped the direction of music psychology into the 21st century.
While the majority of music psychology research has focused on music in a Western context, the field has expanded along with ethnomusicology to examine how the perception and practice of music differs between cultures. It has also emerged into the public sphere. In recent years several bestselling popular science books have helped bring the field into public discussion, notably Daniel Levitin's This Is Your Brain On Music and The World in Six Songs, Oliver Sacks' Musicophilia, and Gary Marcus' Guitar Zero. In addition, the controversial "Mozart effect" sparked lengthy debate among researchers, educators, politicians, and the public regarding the relationship between classical music listening, education, and intelligence.
Research areas
Perception and cognition
Much work within music psychology seeks to understand the cognitive processes that support musical behaviors, including perception, comprehension, memory, attention, and performance. Originally arising in fields of psychoacoustics and sensation, cognitive theories of how people understand music more recently encompass neuroscience, cognitive science, music theory, music therapy, computer science, psychology, philosophy, and linguistics.Affective response
Music has been shown to consistently elicit emotional responses in its listeners, and this relationship between human affect and music has been studied in depth. This includes isolating which specific features of a musical work or performance convey or elicit certain reactions, the nature of the reactions themselves, and how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt. The field draws upon and has significant implications for such areas as philosophy, musicology, and aesthetics, as well the acts of musical composition and performance. The implications for casual listeners are also great; research has shown that the pleasurable feelings associated with emotional music are the result of dopamine release in the striatum—the same anatomical areas that underpin the anticipatory and rewarding aspects of drug addiction.According to research, listening to music has been found to affect the mood of an individual. The main factors in whether it will affect that individual positively or negatively are based on the musics tempo and style. In addition, listening to music also increases cognitive functions, creativity, and decreases feelings of fatigue. All of these factors lead to better workflow and a more optimal result in the activity done while listening to music. This leads to the conclusion that listening to music while performing an activity is an excellent way of increasing productivity and the overall experience.
Neuropsychology
A significant amount of research concerns brain-based mechanisms involved in the cognitive processes underlying music perception and performance. These behaviours include music listening, performing, composing, reading, writing, and ancillary activities. It also is increasingly concerned with the brain basis for musical aesthetics and musical emotion. Scientists working in this field may have training in cognitive neuroscience, neurology, neuroanatomy, psychology, music theory, computer science, and other allied fields, and use such techniques as functional magnetic resonance imaging, transcranial magnetic stimulation, magnetoencephalography, electroencephalography, and positron emission tomography.The cognitive process of performing music requires the interaction of neural mechanisms in both motor and auditory systems. Since every action expressed in a performance produces a sound that influences subsequent expression, this leads to impressive sensorimotor interplay.
Processing pitch
Perceived pitch typically depends on the fundamental frequency, though the dependence could be mediated solely by the presence of harmonics corresponding to that fundamental frequency. The perception of a pitch without the corresponding fundamental frequency in the physical stimulus is called the pitch of the missing fundamental. Neurons lateral to A1 in marmoset monkeys were found to be sensitive specifically to the fundamental frequency of a complex tone, suggesting that pitch constancy may be enabled by such a neural mechanism.Pitch constancy refers to the ability to perceive pitch identity across changes in acoustical properties, such as loudness, temporal envelope, or timbre. The importance of cortical regions lateral to A1 for pitch coding is also supported by studies of human cortical lesions and functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain. These data suggest a hierarchical system for pitch processing, with more abstract properties of sound stimulus processed further along the processing pathways.
''Absolute pitch''
Absolute pitch is defined as the ability to identify the pitch of a musical tone or to produce a musical tone at a given pitch without the use of an external reference pitch. Researchers estimate the occurrence of AP to be 1 in 10,000 people. The extent to which this ability is innate or learned is debated, with evidence for both a genetic basis and for a "critical period" in which the ability can be learned, especially in conjunction with early musical training.Processing rhythm
Behavioural studies demonstrate that rhythm and pitch can be perceived separately, but that they also interact in creating a musical perception. Studies of auditory rhythm discrimination and reproduction in patients with brain injury have linked these functions to the auditory regions of the temporal lobe, but have shown no consistent localization or lateralization. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have shown that the motor regions of the brain contribute to both perception and production of rhythms.Even in studies where subjects only listen to rhythms, the basal ganglia, cerebellum, dorsal premotor cortex and supplementary motor area are often implicated. The analysis of rhythm may depend on interactions between the auditory and motor systems.
Neural correlates of musical training
Although auditory–motor interactions can be observed in people without formal musical training, musicians are an excellent population to study because of their long-established and rich associations between auditory and motor systems. Musicians have been shown to have anatomical adaptations that correlate with their training. Some neuroimaging studies have observed that musicians show lower levels of activity in motor regions than non-musicians during the performance of simple motor tasks, which may suggest a more efficient pattern of neural recruitment.Motor imagery
Previous neuroimaging studies have consistently reported activity in the SMA and premotor areas, as well as in auditory cortices, when non-musicians imagine hearing musical excerpts.Recruitment of the SMA and premotor areas is also reported when musicians are asked to imagine performing.
: an auditory illusion in which two scales are presented with successive tones alternating between each ear but are perceived as simultaneous, unbroken scales.
Psychoacoustics
Psychoacoustics is the scientific study of sound perception. More specifically, it is the branch of science studying the psychological and physiological responses associated with sound. Topics of study include perception of the pitch, timbre, loudness and duration of musical sounds and the relevance of such studies for music cognition or the perceived structure of music; and auditory illusions and how humans localize sound, which can have relevance for musical composition and the design of venues for music performance. Psychoacoustics is a branch of psychophysics.Cognitive musicology
Cognitive musicology is a branch of cognitive science concerned with computationally modeling musical knowledge with the goal of understanding both music and cognition.Cognitive musicology can be differentiated from the fields of music cognition and cognitive neuroscience of music by a difference in methodological emphasis. Cognitive musicology uses computer modeling to study music-related knowledge representation and has roots in artificial intelligence and cognitive science. The use of computer models provides an exacting, interactive medium in which to formulate and test theories.
This interdisciplinary field investigates topics such as the parallels between language and music in the brain. Biologically inspired models of computation are often included in research, such as neural networks and evolutionary programs. This field seeks to model how musical knowledge is represented, stored, perceived, performed, and generated. By using a well-structured computer environment, the systematic structures of these cognitive phenomena can be investigated.
Evolutionary musicology
Evolutionary musicology concerns the "origin of music, the question of animal song, selection pressures underlying music evolution", and "music evolution and human evolution". It seeks to understand music perception and activity in the context of evolutionary theory. Charles Darwin speculated that music may have held an adaptive advantage and functioned as a protolanguage, a view which has spawned several competing theories of music evolution. An alternate view sees music as a by-product of linguistic evolution; a type of "auditory cheesecake" that pleases the senses without providing any adaptive function. This view has been directly countered by numerous music researchers.Cultural differences
An individual's culture or ethnicity plays a role in their music cognition, including their preferences, emotional reaction, and musical memory. Musical preferences are biased toward culturally familiar musical traditions beginning in infancy, and adults' classification of the emotion of a musical piece depends on both culturally specific and universal structural features. Additionally, individuals' musical memory abilities are greater for culturally familiar music than for culturally unfamiliar music.Applied research areas
Many areas of music psychology research focus on the application of music in everyday life as well as the practices and experiences of the amateur and professional musician. Each topic may utilize knowledge and techniques derived from one or more of the areas described above. Such areas include:Music in society
Including:- everyday music listening
- musical rituals and gatherings
- the role of music in forming personal and group identities
- the relation between music and dancing
- social influences on musical preference
Musical preference
Background music
The study of background music focuses on the impact of music with non-musical tasks, including changes in behavior in the presence of different types, settings, or styles of music. In laboratory settings, music can affect performance on cognitive tasks, both positively and negatively. Used extensively as an advertising aid, music may also affect marketing strategies, ad comprehension, and consumer choices. Background music can influence learning, working memory and recall, performance while working on tests, and attention in cognitive monitoring tasks. Background music can also be used as a way to relieve boredom, create positive moods, and maintain a private space. Background music has been shown to put a restless mind at ease by presenting the listener with various melodies and tones.Music in marketing
In both radio and television advertisements, music plays an integral role in content recall, intentions to buy the product, and attitudes toward the advertisement and brand itself. Music's effect on marketing has been studied in radio ads, TV ads, and physical retail settings.One of the most important aspects of an advertisement's music is the "musical fit", or the degree of congruity between cues in the ad and song content. Advertisements and music can be congruous or incongruous for both lyrical and instrumental music. The timbre, tempo, lyrics, genre, mood, as well as any positive or negative associations elicited by certain music should fit the nature of the advertisement and product.
Music and productivity
Several studies have recognized that listening to music while working affects the productivity of people performing complex cognitive tasks. One study suggested that listening to one's preferred genre of music can enhance productivity in the workplace, though other research has found that listening to music while working can be a source of distraction, with loudness and lyrical content possibly playing a role. Other factors proposed to affect the relationship between music listening and productivity include musical structure, task complexity, and degree of control over the choice and use of music.Music education
Including:- optimizing music education
- development of musical behaviors and abilities throughout the lifespan
- the specific skills and processes involved in learning a musical instrument or singing
- activities and practices within a music school
- individual versus group learning of a musical instrument
- the effects of musical education on intelligence
- optimizing practice
Musical aptitude
It is an issue closely related to that of intelligence and IQ, and was pioneered by the work of Carl Seashore. While early tests of aptitude, such as Seashore's The Measurement of Musical Talent, sought to measure innate musical talent through discrimination tests of pitch, interval, rhythm, consonance, memory, etc., later research found these approaches to have little predictive power and to be influenced greatly by the test-taker's mood, motivation, confidence, fatigue, and boredom when taking the test.
Music performance
Including:- the physiology of performance
- music reading and sight-reading, including eye movement
- performing from memory and music-related memory
- acts of improvisation and composition
- flow experiences
- the interpersonal/social aspects of group performance
- music performance quality evaluation by an audience or evaluator, including the influence of musical and non-musical factors
- audio engineering
Music and health
- the effectiveness of music in healthcare and therapeutic settings
- music-specific disorders
- musicians' physical and mental health and well-being
- music performance anxiety
- motivation, burnout, and depression among musicians
- noise-induced hearing loss among musicians
Journals
- Music Perception
- Musicae Scientiae
- Psychology of Music
- Music and Science
- Jahrbuch Musikpsychologie
- Computer Music Journal
- Empirical Musicology Review
- Frontiers in Psychology
- Journal of New Music Research
- Journal of Mathematics and Music
- Journal of the Acoustical Society of America
- Research Studies in Music Education
Societies
- Australian Music & Psychology Society
- European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music
*
Centers of research and teaching
- Music, Sound and Performance Lab, Macquarie University
- Music, Mind and Wellbeing Initiative, Melbourne University
- Empirical Musicology Group, University of New South Wales
- ARC Centre of Excellence for the History of Emotion, University of Western Australia
- The MARCS Institute, University of Western Sydney
- Centre for Systematic Musicology, University of Graz
- Cognitive Psychology Unit, University of Klagenfurt
- Institute for Psychoacoustics and Electronic Music, Ghent University
- Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Music and Media and Technology, McGill University
- Music and Health Research Collaboratory, University of Toronto
- Music Cognition Lab, Queen's University
- Auditory Perception and Music Cognition Research and Training Laboratory, University of Prince Edward Island
- SMART Lab, Ryerson University
- The Music, Acoustics, Perception, and LEarning Lab, McMaster University
- McMaster Institute for Music and the Mind, McMaster University
- BRAMS - International Laboratory for Brain, Music, and Sound Research, University of Montreal and McGill University
- Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, University of Montreal
- Music and Neuroscience Lab, University of Western Ontario
- Center for Music in the Brain, Aarhus University
- Finnish Centre of Excellence in Interdisciplinary Music Research, University of Jyväskylä
- Auditory Cognition and Psychoacoustics team, Claude Bernard University Lyon 1
- University of Burgundy
- IRCAM, Centre Pompidou
- University of Halle-Wittenberg
- Institute for Systematic Musicology, Universität Hamburg
- Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians' Medicine, Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover
- Hanover Music Lab, Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover
- University of Cologne
- University of Oldenburg
- Hochschule für Musik Würzburg
- Technische Universität Chemnitz
- Centre for Music Research, University of Iceland
- University of Limerick
- Kyushu University
- Seoul National University
- Music Cognition Group, University of Amsterdam
- Centre for Music and Health, Norwegian Academy of Music
- Unit of Psychology of Music, Fryderyk Chopin University of Music
- Music Performance and Brain Lab, University of Finance and Management in Warsaw
- Music Cognition Group, Social and Cognitive Computing Department, Institute of High Performance Computing, A*STAR
- Music Technology Group, Pompeu Fabra University
- Speech, Music and Hearing, Royal Institute of Technology
- Music Psychology Group, Uppsala University
- Centre for Music and Science, Cambridge University
- Music and the Human Sciences Group, University of Edinburgh
- Centre for Psychological Research, Keele University
- Music and Science Lab, Durham University
- Interdisciplinary Centre for Scientific Research in Music, University of Leeds
- Social and Applied Psychology Group, University of Leicester
- Music, Mind and Brain Group, Goldsmiths, University College London
- International Music Education Research Centre, UCL Institute of Education, University College London
- Music Cognition Lab, Queen Mary University of London
- Faculty of Music, University of Oxford
- Applied Music Research Centre, University of Roehampton
- Centre for Performance Science, Royal College of Music
- Centre for Music Performance Research, Royal Northern College of Music
- Department of Music, Sheffield University
- Music and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School
- Auditory Perception & Action Lab, University at Buffalo
- Janata Lab, University of California, Davis
- Systematic Musicology Lab, University of California, Los Angeles
- Department of Psychology, University of California, San Diego
- UCSB Music Cognition Lab, University of California, Santa Barbara
- Music Dynamics Lab, University of Connecticut
- The Music Cognition Laboratory, Cornell University
- Music Cognition at Eastman School of Music, University of Rochester
- Center for Music Research, Florida State University
- Music Cognition and Computation Lab, Louisiana State University
- Language and Music Cognition Lab, University of Maryland
- Auditory Cognition and Development Lab, University of Nevada, Las Vegas
- Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, Northwestern University
- Music Theory and Cognition Program, Northwestern University
- Music Cognition Lab, Princeton University
- Cognitive and Systematic Musicology Laboratory, Ohio State University
- Music Learning, Perception, and Cognition Focus Group, University of Oregon
- Center for Computer Research in Music and Acoustics, Stanford University
- Dowling Laboratory, University of Texas at Dallas
- Institute for Music Research, University of Texas at San Antonio
- Laboratory for Music Cognition, Culture & Learning, University of Washington
- Music, Imaging, and Neural Dynamics Laboratory, Wesleyan University
- Brain Research and Interdisciplinary Neurosciences Lab, Western Michigan University
Encyclopedia entries
- Palmer, Caroline & Melissa K. Jungers : Music Cognition. In: Lynn Nadel: Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Vol. 3, London: Nature Publishing Group, pp. 155–158.
- Deutsch, Diana : Music. In Oxford Bibliographies in Music. Edited by Dunn, D.S. New York: Oxford University Press. 2013,
- Thompson, William Forde : "Music in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, An Encyclopedia". Sage Publications Inc., New York.
Introductory reading
- Day, Kingsley. Observer online.
- Jourdain, Robert. Music, the Brain, and Ecstasy: How Music Captures Our Imagination. New York: William Morrow and Company..
- Honing, Henkjan. New Brunswick, N.J.: Transaction Publishers..
- Levitin, D. J.. New York: Dutton.
- Margulis, Elizabeth Hellmuth.. ' New York, NY: Oxford University Press..
- Margulis, Elizabeth Hellmuth.. ' New York, NY: Oxford University Press..
- Snyder, Bob. "Music and Memory: an introduction" The MIT Press..
- J.P.E. Harper-Scott and Jim Samson 'An Introduction to Music Studies', Chapter 4: John Rink,The Psychology of Music,, pp. 60.
Advanced reading
- Deutsch, D. . The Psychology of Music, 1st Edition. New York: Academic Press..
- Deutsch, D. . The Psychology of Music, 2nd Edition. San Diego: Academic Press..
- Deutsch, D. . The Psychology of Music, 3rd Edition. San Diego: Academic Press..
- Dowling, W. Jay and Harwood, Dane L.. Music Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press..
- Hallam, Cross, & Thaut, . The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Krumhansl, Carol L.. Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press..
- Patel, Anirrudh D.. . New York: Oxford University Press.
- Parncutt, R.. Berlin: Springer.
- Sloboda, John A.. The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press..
- Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. The MIT Press..
- Jackendoff, Ray : Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Chapter 11: Levels of Musical Structure, section 11.1: What is Musical Cognition?
- Temperley, D.. The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures. The MIT Press..
- Thompson, W. F.. Music, Thought, and Feeling: Understanding the Psychology of Music New York: Oxford University Press..
- Zbikowski, Lawrence M.. Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis. Oxford University Press, USA..
- North, A.C. & Hargreaves, D.J.. The Social and Applied Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press..