Unami language


Unami is an Algonquian language spoken by the Lenape people in the late 17th century and the early 18th century, in what then was the southern two-thirds of New Jersey, southeastern Pennsylvania and the northern two-thirds of Delaware, but later in Ontario and Oklahoma. It is one of the two Delaware languages, the other being Munsee. The last fluent speaker in the United States, Edward Thompson, of the Delaware Tribe of Indians, died on August 31, 2002. His sister Nora Thompson Dean provided valuable information about the language to linguists and other scholars.
"Lenni-Lenape," literally means "Men of Men", but is translated to mean "Original People." The Lenape names for the areas they inhabited were Scheyichbi, which means "water's edge", and Lenapehoking, meaning "in the land of the Delaware Indians." It describes the ancient homeland of all Delaware Indians, both Unami and Munsee. The English named the river running through much of the traditional range of the Lenape after the first governor of the Jamestown Colony, Lord De La Warr, and consequently referred to the people who lived around the river as "Delaware Indians".

History

Unami is an Eastern Algonquian language. The hypothetical common ancestor language from which the Eastern Algonquian languages descend is Proto-Eastern Algonquian. An intermediate group Delawarean that is a descendant of Proto-Eastern Algonquian consists of Mahican and Common Delaware, the latter being a further subgroup comprising Munsee Delaware and Unami Delaware. The justification for Delawarean as an intermediate subgroup rests upon the high degree of similarity between Mahican and the two Delaware languages, but relatively little detailed argumentation in support of Delawarean has been adduced.
Compared to Munsee, Unami has undergone extensive phonological innovation, coupled with morphological regularization.
The PEA vowel system consisted of four long vowels *i·, *o·, *e·, *a·, and two short vowels *a and ə. The vowel history is as follows: *i·, *o·, *e·, and *a·, and *a. This system was continued down to Common Delaware, but Munsee and Unami have innovated separately with respect to the vowel systems.

Revival attempts

The dominant modern version of the Southern Unami dialect called "Lenape" is taught by the Delaware Tribe of Indians, headquartered in Bartlesville, Oklahoma. They have the Lenape Language Preservation Project. The same dialect was spoken by the Delaware Nation in Anadarko in the southwestern part of Oklahoma. Both Oklahoma and Delaware tribes have recorded native speakers and produced written lessons for instruction, which are available for sale from Various Indian Peoples Publishing Company, this company is in Texas. These efforts, in conjunction with other community efforts comprise an attempt to preserve the language.
Some descriptions of the Northern Unami dialect as spoken during the 18th century are given by Moravian missionary John Heckewelder. Many large works in the Northern Unami dialect have been edited, translated into English, and published by Lenape Texts & Studies.

Phonology

Consonants

Unami has been analyzed as having contrastive geminate and non-geminate obstruent consonants, although this contrast is relatively weak. A full analysis of the status of the geminates, also known as long consonants, is not available, and more than one analysis of Delaware consonants has been proposed. The long consonants are described as having low functional yield, and they differentiate relatively few pairs of words but occur in contrasting environments. Some examples of contrastive geminate pairs include: k·ə́ntka·n "then you danced" versus kə́ntka·n "then there was dancing"; p·ɔ́·m "his thigh" versus pɔ́·m "the ham"; and nsa·s·a·k·ənə́mən "I stuck it out repeatedly" versus nsa·sa·k·ənə́mən "I stuck it out slowly".
There are also rules that lengthen consonants in certain environments. The length mark is used to indicate gemination of a preceding consonant or vowel length, although in the literature on Unami the raised dot is often used for these purposes, as other diacritics may be used above vowels.

In the following chart, the usual transcription used in the sources is given with the IPA in brackets.
BilabialDentalPostalveolarVelarGlottal
Plosivep t č k
Fricatives š x h
Nasalm n
Laterall
Semivowelw j

Vowels

Unami vowels are presented as organized into contrasting long–short pairs. One asymmetry is that high short is paired with long, and the pairing of long and short is noteworthy. It is worth noting that /ə/ and /o/ are not distinguishable before /w/, /m/, and /kw/. Additionally, vowels are classified as strong and weak, which plays an important role in determining stress. Long vowels and vowels before consonant clusters are automatically strong. Certain short vowels, which are differentiated with a breve – /ĭ, ĕ, ŏ/ – are also strong vowels because they are treated morphophonemically as long vowels, even though they are pronounced as short. In a sequence of syllables containing a short vowel followed by a consonant or consonant and /w/, the odd-numbered vowels are weak, and the even numbered vowels are strong.
Furthermore, some short vowels are strong even in a weakening environment; such exceptions are often marked with a grave accent. Additionally, some vowels which are unaffected by predicted vowel syncope are marked with an acute accent. There is a predictable tendency, additionally, to nasalize and lengthen a vowel before /ns/ and /nš/, so that /lowé·nso/ is realized closer to from underlying /ələwe·nsəw/.
FrontCentralBack
Close
Mid
Open

FrontCentralBack
Close
Mid
Open

Syllable structure

Syllable structure is diverse, permitting a certain amount of consonant clustering. The following consonant clusters can occur:
Additionally, certain consonants may combine with the semivowel /w/. Note that some underlying forms may also contain /sw/ and /šw/, but these are always removed by morphophonemic processes.
is generally predictable in Unami. The rightmost nonfinal strong vowel is stressed, or a strong vowel in final position if it is the only one in the word. Often when stress would be expected to fall on the antepenult it is shifted to the penult. This change is found in three conjunct endings: /-ak/, /-at/, and /-an/. In the last case, the accent shifts to the penultimate /-an/ only if it would otherwise fall on an antepenultimate short vowel, and if the consonant between them is voiced.

Phonological processes

Unami phonology is extremely complex, with various morphophonological rules, and a theoretical form usually undergoes a set of predictable phonological processes to produce the true form found in speech. There are about 17 such rules common to both Munsee and Unami, and another 28 unique to Unami, though this analysis ignores predictable exceptions, such as the class of static words which may skip many of these rules. These rules govern things such as consonant lengthening/shortening, vowel syncopation, metathesis, vowel coloring, etc.
A list of processes unique to Unami follow. Note that these are written in linguistic notation. Thus, → ∅ / _V when are weak means that the sounds /ə/ and /a/ become null in the context of when they are weak and appear before either /h/ or /x/ and another vowel. The slash means "in the context of", and the underscore _ indicates where the /ə/ or /a/ must occur. In some notations the pound symbol appears, indicating word boundaries. Regular parenthesis indicate optional conditions when framing phonemes or additional information about phonemes: "C=stop". The capital letters C, V, and N mean "consonant," "vowel," and "nasal" respectively.

Nouns

Third person participants are marked for gender, obviation, and presence. Generally, the inanimate, obviative, and absentative categories are more marked than their opposites, but it is not clear whether animacy or inanimacy is the more marked of the opposition. The first and second persons are not marked for presence or obviation and are always animate.

Obviation

The first mentioned and/or primary animate third person is proximate; all other third persons are obviative, unless they act in conjunction with the proximate participant. Verbs are also inflected to indicate whether the verbal action is proximate on obviate or obviate on proximate.

Presence

Third-person participants can be marked by a special set of endings indicating their absence from the general area of the focus of discourse. For example, absentative endings are used when speaking of the deceased, as in the sentence no·lăčahko·ná·na nkahe·səná·na mother, in which both verb and noun are marked with the absentative /-a/ ending.

Gender

Nouns in Unami are classified as animate or inanimate, which is reflected in verbal conjugation. Animate nouns denote human beings, animals, spirits, trees, and certain fruits, tubers, root vegetables, and other unpredictable exceptions like /ko·n/ and /nhíkaš/. Thus, /té·hi·m/, /xáskwi·m/, /ke·skúnthak/, /mpi/, and /nhíka·t/ are inanimate, while /lə́nu/, /xho·k/, /mahtán'tu/ and /hɔ́pəni·s/ are animate. However, traditionally inanimate nouns which are directly addressed or personified are treated as animate. Thus, traditionally inanimate /ăsǝ́n/ is treated as animate in the sentence šá·i a· ăsǝ́nak kǝnčí·mowak.

Verbs

Unami is a highly agglutinative, polysynthetic language. Verbs in Unami are marked for person and number, and contain inflectional elements of order, aspect, and the negative.
A table of the personal pronouns is given below. The first person plural may be either inclusive or exclusive.
Following are tables exemplifying verbal paradigms in Unami in the independent order, indicative mood and present tense.

Animate Intransitive (AI) Verbs

Transitive Animate (TA) Verbs

Inanimate Intransitive (II) Verbs

Transitive Inanimate (TI) Verbs

The TI themes have the same inflection as AI stems for all conjuncts Three forms are illustrated from each type.

Affixes

Prefixes

Verbal prefixes are used only in the independent order, although some forms of the independent order lack a prefix. There are three of them: /n-/, /k-/, and /w-/. If a stem has an underlying initial vowel, a /t/ is inserted after the prefix, and before this and other stem-initial consonants a /ə/ is inserted. Sometimes, this /ə/ contracts with a stem-initial /wə/ to /o·/ except when the /o·/ would be phonetically shortened via rule U-4a. Examples include: /ntá·mwi/ versus /á·mwi·/. Two roots with initial /t/ extend the syllable with /-ən/ when adding prefixes; these roots are /tal-/ and /tax-/, e.g. náni ntəntala·wsí·ne·n from the animate intransitive stem /tala·wəsi·/.
Prefixes are mutually exclusive and are selected based on the following rule: if one of the participants is second person, the second person prefix is used; if not, if one of the participants is the first person, then the first person prefix is used; if none of these applies, other forms, if they take a prefix, take the third person prefix. This is the well-known Algonquian 2-1-3 precedence rule.

Suffixes

Suffixes are grouped into eight positional classes. These are:
The first position is filled only for transitive verbs and help describe the relationship between the two participants by indicating which is the agent and which is the object. The direct and inverse theme signs indicate the direction of the verb along a spectrum what might be called distance. From least to most distant the participants are: first or second; indefinite ; proximate third person; obviative third person; farther obviative third person; inanimate. If the subject is less distant than the object, the direct theme is used; if the subject is more distant, the inverse signs are used. After transitive animate verb stems appear one of the four following theme signs:
For transitive inanimate verbs, there appears the theme sign pertaining to the appropriate TI class:
For Class 2 TI theme signs, in certain derivational categories, the theme sign is retained even when the thematic element is dropped. The contrast between both categories is sharper in the Central Algonquian languages, whose theme sign has a more complex series of alternants.
The second position consists of diminutives in ₂/-tī/ and pejoratives in ₂/-šī/ An example of a diminutive is /pé·t·o/, contrasted with the pejorative /pé·šo/. The use of /t/ in the formation of diminutives seems to be an innovation of Unami, as many other Algonquian languages use */s/ or */ʃ/.
In position three are the affixes ₃/-lī/, which marks the obviative third person, and ₃/-htī/, which marks the plural.
Position four contains the negative affix ₄/-w/ and prohibitive imperative and future imperative forms, which have complex series of alternants.
Position five contains the central endings which index the central participant of each form, except those using TA theme signs ₁/-i·/ and ₁/-əl/;
Position six contains the affix endings: /-pan/ marks the preterite, and /sa/ ~ /shan/ mark the present.
Position seven contains peripheral endings, which are used to mark the nominal category of some 3rd person participants in forms in the independent and conjunct orders.
Position eight reflects the subjunctive, prohibitive, and future modes.

Syntax

Unami is, like many Algonquian languages, polysynthetic and highly agglutinative. This means that most of the information is encoded in the verb, making word order more fluid than in English. Unfortunately, syntax is one of the least studied aspects of the Unami language; there is much more data on morphology, because of an especial focus on reconstructing Proto-Algonquian.
Some examples of complex sentences in Unami include: