Causative alternation


In linguistics, causative alternation is a phenomenon in which certain verbs that express a change of state can be used transitively or intransitively. A causatively alternating verb, such as "open", has both a transitive meaning and an intransitive meaning. When causatively alternating verbs are used transitively they are called causatives since, in the transitive use of the verb, the subject is causing the action denoted by the intransitive version. When causatively alternating verbs are used intransitively, they are referred to as anticausatives or inchoatives because the intransitive variant describes a situation in which the theme participant undergoes a change of state, becoming, for example, "opened".

Basic example

Example of the causative alternation with the English verb 'break':
The general structure of the causative and anticausative variants of the causative alternation in English:
The causative alternation is a transitivity alternation. The verb “break” demonstrates causative alternation because it can alternate between transitive and intransitive use and the transitive alternate “John broke the vase’' indicates the cause of the intransitive alternate “the vase broke.” In other words, the transitive use denotes that it was John that caused the vase to break. The causative alternative has an external argument, which bears the theta role agent which is not present in the intransitive alternative. The object of the causative alternative bears the same thematic role of theme as the subject of the anticausative alternative.

Principal characteristics

Cross-linguistically, the verbs that participate in the causative alternation are anticausatives which denote movement or a change of state or degree.

Anticausatives

Under one possible and fairly common analysis, unaccusatives and unergatives form the two subclasses of intransitive verbs. Unaccusative verbs cannot assign case to their deep-structure object which bears the theme/patient thematic role; because of this, the object moves to the subject position in the surface form in order to obtain case in accordance with Burzio's generalization. The movement of "the book" from object position to subject position is traced in example. Therefore, unaccusative verbs take a semantic theme or patient subject. On the other hand, unergative verbs take a semantic agent or initiator subject.
Most unaccusative verbs participate in the causative alternation. The unaccusatives that do causatively alternate are anticausative verbs which make up a subclass of unaccusative verbs called alternating unaccusatives. The other subclass of unaccusative verbs, pure unaccusatives, consists of all other unaccusatives that do not take part in causative alternation.
Though some unaccusative verbs can undergo causative alternation, it is never the case that an unergative can.

Change of state verbs

In various languages, it is seen that the verbs participating in the causative alternation are verbs that denote movement or a change of state or degree. However, not all change of state verbs are anticausatives and therefore, not all change of state verbs participate in the causative alternation. For instance, a change of state verb like 'bloom' does not show causative alternation as it is a pure unnacusative. It is possible to say that "The cactus bloomed," but it is ungrammatical to say that "The warm weather bloomed the cactus."

Theoretical approaches

The general consensus in the field is that there is a derivational relationship between verbs undergoing the causative alternation that share the same lexical entry. From this it follows that there is uncertainty surrounding which form, the intransitive or the transitive, is the base from which the other is derived. Another matter of debate is whether the derivation takes place at the syntactic or lexical level.
With reference to these assumptions, syntactic and lexicalist accounts have been proposed. These approaches account for intransitive, transitive and common base approaches.
The intransitive base approaches, also known as causativization, state that the transitive variant is derived from the intransitive variant by adding one argument, that is an agent. The transitive base approaches, also known as decausativization, propose that the intransitive form is derived from the transitive by deleting one argument that is the agent. Common base approaches suggest that both the transitive and the intransitive forms are formulated from a common base.

Lexicalist

According to the intransitive base/causativization approach, the intransitive form is the base and a causative predicate is added to the Lexical Conceptual Structure in order to make the verb transitive. In the following example, the basic LCS, “The stick broke.” is embedded under a cause predicate, in this case “Katherine,” to form the derived LCS “Katherine broke the stick.
In, “x” is the variable, and the CHANGE operator refers to the change-of-state. In the anticausative “the stick” undergoes the change “break”, namely, the stick breaks. Moreover, the “y” variable refers to “Katherine” and the CAUSE operator refers to the cause of the change. In the causative,, it is “Katherine” who causes the action “break”, and is therefore the cause operator.
The transitive/decausativation approach, assumes a lexical operation which performs precisely the opposite of the causativization approach discussed above.
In this approach, according to the following rule, the intransitive/anticausative form is derived from the transitive/causative form by deleting the cause predicate from the LCS. In example below, the LCS is “Katherine broke the stick” and the cause predicate “Katherine” is deleted.
The stick broke.

Syntactic

Under a syntactic intransitive base approach, the transitive form is derived from the intransitive form by insertion of a verbal layer projected by a head expressing causation and introducing the external agent argument. This idea assumes that a verbal phrase is able to be separated into different layers of verbal projections whereby each of the layers provide a specifier where an argument can be attached. In addition, the layers are joined together by head movement of the lowest verb head to positions higher in the syntactic structure. Change-of-state verbs are broken-down into the verbal layers of initiation phrase, process phrase and result phrase, which approximately correspond to the predicate cause, become, and state respectively.
Example, the anticausative variant, is basic according to the intransitive base approach. The theme is initially merged into the specifier of resP and that it then moves to the specifier of procP. The theme, is therefore given a complex theta-role of both the resultee and the undergoer of the event. In the syntax, the causative form is derived through the addition of an init-head, which introduces the external initiator argument in example.

Connecting the lexical and syntactic analyses

The syntactic and lexical analyses correspond in the following ways:
In the lexical accounts, the causative alternation takes place at the level of the lexical conceptual structure, while in the syntactic accounts, the alternation happens at the level of the syntax, as a result of the interaction between the syntactic structure and the basic verbal element.
In the lexical accounts corresponds with the layered process phrase and the result phrase in the syntactic account. The y CAUSE [x CHANGE in the lexical accounts corresponds with the process phrase, the result P along with initiator phrase, which is the additional verbal layer in the syntactic account. The presence of this additional verbal layer is what distinguishes the causative/transitive variant from the anticausative/instransitive variant in the syntactic account. In contrast, in the lexical accounts, the causative is determined by the presence of a causative predicate.

Child [language acquisition

Children typically begin to generate causatively alternating verbs around the age of 1;11. Around this time the causative alternations closely resemble an adult-like form; however, around the age of 2;6 to 12;0 children begin making common errors of overregularization, in which they erroneously overuse the causative. Children often acquire the syntactic pattern that goes along with verbal alternations; however, that does not mean that they acquire the lexical semantic restrictions that accompany these alternations. Three common overregulizations include:
In language acquisition, once children establish that a particular transitivity alternation is productive, they often extend the alternation to new verbs, despite the argument structure of the individual verb. It has been suggested that causative alternation errors come from three sources:
When acquiring causatively alternating verbs children must learn both the semantic representation and the argument structure of each verb to produce grammatical sentences It has been suggested that children learn this is through the no negative evidence problem; for example a child will learn that the verb ‘throw’ can never be used in a subject position: *”the ball threw”.

In children with specific language impairments

Children with specific language impairments tend to produce less mature responses and fewer mature responses compared to children of the same age comparison. The children with SLI produced slightly fewer overgeneralizations, but in general, did not appear to differ in frequency or type of overgeneralizations when compared to the AC children. In English, children need to be able to organize verbs into three separate syntactic groups in order to properly use causative alternations. These syntactic groups include:
  1. Fixed intransitives
  2. Fixed transitives
  3. Causatives
While children with SLI can typically use the lexical alternation for causative alternation as well as AC children, they tend to have difficulty using the syntactic cues to deal with verbs with fixed transitivity.

Examples

Indo-European languages

In many Indo-European languages, causative alternation regularly involves the use of a reflexive pronoun, clitic, or affix in the inchoative use of the verb. A reflexive pronoun is a pronoun that is preceded by the noun, adjective, adverb or pronoun to which it refers within the same clause.

French

French is a Romance language which incorporates the use of a reflexive pronoun with a verb's inchoative form.
Seen in is the causative use of the verb "briser", conjugated in present tense.
Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb.
Note the use of the reflexive pronoun “se” in, which is required for the sentence to be grammatically correct in French. When the reflexive pronoun is not present, the sentence is ungrammatical.

Italian

Italian is another Romance language that, like French, incorporates the use of a reflexive pronoun with a verb's inchoative form.
Seen in is the causative use of the verb "chiudere" conjugated in past tense.

Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb.
Note the use of the reflexive pronoun “si” in, which behaves in the same manner as the French “se” shown in example.

German

It is common for languages to use a reflexive marker to signal the inchoative member of an alternating pair of verbs. Inchoative verbs in German are marked either by the reflexive pronoun “sich”, or not marked at all.
Shown in examples and is a verb that alternates without any use of a reflexive pronoun:
Seen in is the causative use of the verb "zerbrechen", conjugated in past tense.

Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb.

Seen in examples and is an example of a verb that requires a reflexive pronoun to denote the anticausative:
Seen in is the causative use of the verb "öffnen", conjugated in past tense. There is no reflexive pronoun present, it is not needed in the causative use.

Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb.

Note the use of the reflexive pronoun "sich" in, which behaves in the same manner as French "se" and Italian "si" seen above in examples and.

Asian languages

Japanese

In Japanese, causative alternation is seen in ergative verbs and paired verbs. Ergative verbs are verbs that can be transitive or intransitive without morphological change, while paired verbs are verbs that require morphological changes in order to be read as transitive or intransitive.
An example of an ergative verb in Japanese is shown below in examples and :
Seen in is the causative use of the verb "開く" - "hiraku", conjugated in past tense.
Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb.
Note that in and, the form of the verb does not undergo any sort of morphological change, making "開く" an ergative verb.
An example of a paired verb in Japanese is shown below in examples and :
In, the verb "to drop" takes on the verb form "落とす" - "otosu", conjugated in past tense. The verb "落とす" requires an agent to physically drop an object.
"Heiji dropped the coin."
In, the verb "to drop" takes on the verb form "落ちる" - "ochiru", conjugated in past tense. The verb "落ちる" is used when there is no agent in the sentence to do the physical dropping of the coin itself.

Chinese

Mandarin Chinese is a language that lacks inflectional morphology that marks tense, case, agreement, or lexical category. The language also does not have derivational morphology to mark the transitivity of verbs. Instead, Mandarin Chinese uses verbal compounding to do causative alternation.
Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb "碎".
The following examples and show an ungrammatical use of the causative alternative of the verb "碎".

Korean

Causative alternation in Korean is difficult to interpret. There have been many attempts to capture the restrictions on Korean causative alternation, but none of them capture the restrictions entirely.
Some verbs in Korean bear similarities to the paired verbs in Japanese. Morphological changes take place in order to show transitivity and intransitivity.
Shown in is the causative use of the verb "열다" - "yeolda", conjugated in past tense.
Shown in is the anticausative use of the verb, conjugated in past tense.
In examples and, it is seen that the infinitive forms of the verb "yeolda" are the same, but causative and anticausative forms take on different conjugated forms in order to show causativity.
Korean also bears similarities to Chinese in its verbal compounding.
Shown in and is an example of a verb that requires compounding in order to be grammatical in the causative use.
Seen in is the anticausative use of the verb "죽다" - "jugda", conjugated in past tense.
Seen in is the causative use of the verb, conjugated in past tense.
Example shows that the verb "jugda" behaves similarly to the verb "po" in Mandarin Chinese seen in example in that the verb requires some sort of action performed by the agent.