Causative
In linguistics, a causative is a valency-increasing operation that indicates that a subject either causes someone or something else to do or be something or causes a change in state of a non-volitional event. Normally, it brings in a new argument, A, into a transitive clause, with the original subject S becoming the object O.
All languages have ways to express causation but differ in the means. Most, if not all, languages have specific or lexical causative forms. Some languages also have morphological devices that change verbs into their causative forms or change adjectives into verbs of becoming. Other languages employ periphrasis, with control verbs, idiomatic expressions or auxiliary verbs. There tends to be a link between how "compact" a causative device is and its semantic meaning.
The normal English causative verb or control verb used in periphrasis is make rather than cause. Linguistic terms are traditionally given names with a Romance root, which has led some to believe that cause is more prototypical. While cause is a causative, it carries some additional meaning and is less common than make. Also, while most other English causative verbs require a to complement clause, make does not require one, at least when it is not being used in the passive voice.
Terminology
Many authors have written extensively on causative constructions and have used a variety of terms, often to talk about the same things.S, A, and O are terms used in morphosyntactic alignment to describe arguments in a sentence. The subject of an intransitive verb is S, the agent of a transitive verb is A, and the object of a transitive is O. These terms are technically not abbreviations for "subject", "agent", and "object", though they can usually be thought of that way. P is often used instead of O in many works.
The term underlying is used to describe sentences, phrases, or words that correspond to their causative versions. Often, this underlying sentence may not be explicitly stated. For example, for the sentence "'John made Bill drive the truck'", the underlying sentence would be Bill drove the truck. This has also been called the base situation.
A derived sentence would be the causativized variant of the underlying sentence.
The causer is the new argument in a causative expression that causes the action to be done. The causer is the new argument brought into a derived sentence. In the example sentence above, John is the causer.
The causee is the argument that actually does the action in a causativized sentence. It is usually present in both the underlying and derived sentences. Bill is the causee in the above example.
Devices
There are various ways of encoding causation, which form somewhat of a continuum of "compactness."Lexical
Lexical causatives are common in the world's languages. There are three kinds of lexical causatives, the unifying factor being that the idea of causation is part of the semantics of the verb itself.On the surface, lexical causatives look essentially the same as a regular transitive verb. There are a few reasons why this is not true. The first is that transitive verbs generally do not have an intransitive counterpart but lexical causatives do. The semantics of the verbs show the difference as well. A regular transitive verb implies a single event while a lexical causative implies a realization of an event:
Sentence is judged ungrammatical because it goes against the successful event implied by the verb melt.
One word
Some languages, including English, have ambitransitive verbs like break, burn or awake, which may either be intransitive or transitiveThese are split into two varieties: agentive and patientive ambitransitives. Agentive ambitransitives include verbs such as walk and knit because the S of the intransitive corresponds to the A of the transitive. For example:
This type of ambitransitive does not show a causative relationship.
For patientive ambitransitives, such as trip and spill, the S of the intransitive corresponds to the O of the transitive:
These are further divided into two more types, based on speakers' intuition. Some, like spill in, are primarily transitive and secondarily intransitive. Other verbs like this include smash and extend. Other verbs, such as trip in go the other way: they are primarily intransitive and secondarily transitive.
Other examples of this type include explode, melt, dissolve, walk, and march. It is this type of ambitransitive verb that is considered a causative This is given some anecdotal evidence in that to translate above into languages with morphological causatives, a morpheme would need to be attached to the verb.
Lexical causatives are apparently constrained to involving only one agentive argument. Semantically, the causer is usually marked as the patient. In fact, it's unlikely whether any language has a lexical causative for verbs such as "swim," "sing," "read," or "kick."
Irregular stem change
English fell can be thought of as a lexical causative of fall, exemplifying this category. This is considered a lexical change because it is not at all productive. If it were productive, it would be an internal change morphological causative.Two words
English has verb pairs such as rise and raise, eat and feed, see and show where one is essentially the causative correspondent of the other.These pairs are linked semantically by various means, usually involving translation. For example, burn, as in The grass burned, would translate as awa- in Yimas while burn, as in I burned the grass, would translate as ampu- in Yimas
Morphological
There are eight different morphological processes by which a causative may be marked, roughly organized by compactness:Process | Basic Verb | Causative Form | Language |
internal change | tìkti 'be suitable' | táikyti 'make suitable' | Lithuanian |
tone change | nɔ̂ 'be awake' | nɔ̄ 'awaken, rouse' | Lahu |
consonant repetition | xarab 'go bad' | xarrab 'make go bad, ruin' | Gulf Arabic |
vowel lengthening | mar 'die' | ma:r 'kill' | Kashmiri |
reduplication | bengok 'shout' | be-bengok 'make shout' | Javanese |
prefix | gǝbba 'enter' | a-gǝbba 'insert' | Amharic |
suffix | -kam- 'die' | -kam-isa- 'kill' | K'iche' |
circumfix | -č'am- 'eat' | -a-č'm-ev- 'feed ' | Georgian |
Within morphological causatives, this degree of compactness bears an important variable when considering the semantics of the two processes. For example, mechanisms that do not change the length of the word are shorter than those that lengthen it. Of those that lengthen it, shorter changes are more compact than longer.
Verbs can be classified into four categories, according to how susceptible they are to morphological causativization:
- Inactive intransitives
- Middle/ingestive verbs
- Active intransitives
- Transitive verbs
Two verbs in one predicate
A number of languages involve a form of analytic causative that involves two verbs in a single predicate, such as French, Spanish, Italian and Catalan. For example, when French faire is used as a causative, the causee noun phrase cannot occur between it and the next verb.Unlike most other Romance languages, Portuguese uses a periphrastic construction like that of English, discussed below.
Kiowa uses a similar mechanism. Verbs can be compounded with the transitive verb ɔ́m to create a causative:
Periphrastic constructions
Some languages use a periphrastic construction to express causation and typically include two verbs and two clauses. English causatives prototypically use make in the main clause with the lexical verb in a subordinate clause, as in "I made him go."Other languages, such as Persian, have the opposite syntax: the causative is in a subordinating clause and the main verb is in the main clause, as in the following example from Macushi:
Canela-Krahô has a combination of the two in which the causee is marked twice, once in each clause:
Portuguese also has a periphrastic construction like that of English but unlike most other Romance languages:
Analytic causatives are sometimes not considered to be valency increasing devices, but they can semantically be interpreted as such.
Semantics
A language may have one or more different formal mechanisms for expression causation. For languages with only one, the semantic range is broad. For those with multiple, there is always a semantic difference between the two. R.M.W. Dixon breaks down these semantic differences into 9 parameters, involving the verb itself, the causee, and the causer:These parameters are not mutually exclusive. Many causative constructions involve the semantics of two or more parameters. However, the difference between the causatives in a language most likely will be distinguished by one of the parameters.
Relationship between devices and semantics
[Animacy] of the object
There is a strong correlation between the semantics of a causative and the mechanism by which it is expressed. Generally, if a causative is more "compact" than another, it usually implies a more direct causation.So, for inanimate and unconscious objects, English analytic causatives are not completely synonymous with lexical causatives :
- "I made the tree fall."
- "I made the chicken die."
- "I made the cup rise to my lips."
- "I felled the tree."
- "I killed the chicken."
- "I raised the cup to my lips."
For animate and conscious objects, there is a different difference in meaning:
- "He caused them to lie down."
- "He laid them down."
Finite and non-finite verbs
Generally, the more distance between the causer and the causee, the more finite the verb. Consider the following examples from Spanish:The first example implies that Montezuma was physically there and was directly involved in making Cortés eat bread. The second example implies that Montezuma was not physically there and arranged for something to happen to make Cortés eat bread, perhaps by killing all of his cattle. That could approximate the English construction "Montezuma got Cortés to eat bread." Therefore, at least in Spanish, a conjugated verb implies a more direct causation.
Dixon's prototypes
Dixon examines this correlation cross-linguistically, and summarizes his findings in the following table. In this table, L refers to lexical causatives, M1 refers to more compact morphological processes while M2 refers to less compact processes, CP refers to complex predicates, and P refers to periphrastic constructions. These processes are explained more clearly in the devices section above.Parameter 9, Involvement, cannot be included in the table because the only two languages with this distinction, Nomatsiguenga and Kamayurá, the morphemes are about the same length. When a larger sample of languages show this distinction, perhaps this parameter can be included in the table.
The table shows that for each of eight semantic parameters outlined in the semantics section above, more compact causative processes show one distinction while less compact processes show the other distinction. For example, Parameter 6 distinguishes between more direct and less direct causation. In Hindi, M1, or the shorter morphological process, shows direct causation while M2, the longer morphological process, shows indirect causation.
Summarizing the table, Dixon has given two prototypes for causatives:
All eight of the components in each prototype are never attested in a single causative. However, a single process may have two or three components. Dixon admits to these being very tentative and in need for further investigation.
Syntax
also outlines the syntactic possibilities of causatives in the world's languages.Intransitives
Since intransitive verbs have low valency, virtually any type of causative construction can apply to them productively within a language. Some constructions are only allowed with intransitive verbs and some languages only allow causatives of intransitive verbs, with some exceptions. In all cases, the original subject of the underlying intransitive verb corresponds with the object of the derived transitive verb. All languages have this construction, though some allow a semantic difference if the original subject is marked differently.For split systems, causatives of intransitives may be treated differently.
The syntax of a causative construction is almost always the same as some other type of sentence, such as a sentence with a transitive verb. Tariana, however, is an exception to this rule.
Transitives
In the causative of a transitive verb, the new causer always becomes the new A of the sentence. What happens to the causee and the original object depend on the language. Dixon shows that there are five main types of situations:Within type there are two main subtypes. Either the original A goes into the first empty slot in a hierarchy or it always takes a certain function.
For the first subtype, there is a hierarchy involved in the language:
French is a language that follows this hierarchy. When a causative is employed, the original A does not get marked the same for intransitives, transitives, and ditransitives. In this first example, the verb in intransitive, and with the subject slot taken, the original A becomes a direct object:
The following example has a transitive verb. The subject and direct object slots are filled so the original A becomes an indirect object:
This final French example has a ditransitive verb. The subject is je, the direct object is une lettre, and the indirect object is directeur, so the original A is marked as an oblique:
While some writers have called this hierarchical causative construction the norm, outside of Romance languages it is in fact rather rare.
Most other languages are of the second subtype of type, and the original A takes on a set case or marking, regardless whether the underlying verb is intransitive or transitive:
Ditransitives
The syntactic and morphological constraints of individual language generally restrict causatives of ditransitive verbs. The underlying phrase already contains an A, O, and indirect object, and so in order to accommodate a fourth argument, languages employ a variety of constructions. They tend to be idiosyncratic and are difficult to group together into types. Additionally, data is patchy for many languages since descriptions of languages seldom include information of causatives of ditransitives.Double causatives
Some types of causative constructions essentially do not permit double causatives, e.g. it would be difficult to find a lexical double causative. Periphrastic causatives however, have the potential to always be applied iteratively.Many Indo-Aryan languages have lexical double causatives.
For morphological causatives, some languages do not allow single morpheme to be applied twice on a single verb while others do, though sometimes with an idiomatic meaning. Other languages, such as Nivkh, have two different morphological mechanisms that can apply to a single verb. Still others have one morpheme that applies to intransitives and another to transitives. All of these examples apply to underlying intransitive verbs, yielding a ditransitive verb. So far, there are no reliable data for a morphological double causative of a transitive verb, resulting in a verb with four arguments.
Other topics
Causative (repetitive)
, an indigenous language spoken in California, has a morpheme, -lsaˑ, that indicates causation in addition to repetition. This is separate from the language's normal mechanisms of causation.This implies a single act by the causer, but multiple acts by the causee.
Causative voice
The causative voice is a grammatical voice promoting the oblique argument of a transitive verb to an actor argument. When the causative voice is applied to a verb, its valency increases by one. If, after the application of the grammatical voice, there are two actor arguments, one of them is obligatorily demoted to an oblique argument.Japanese, Turkish and Mongolian are examples of languages with the causative voice. The following are examples from Japanese:
Causal case
The causal or causative case is a grammatical case that indicates that the marked noun is the cause or reason for something. It is found in the Dravidian languages Kannada and Telugu, the Native South American language Quechua, and Northeast Caucasian Archi. It is also found in extinct Tocharian B, an Indo-European language.Causal-final case
The causal-final is a grammatical case in Hungarian expressing the meaning 'for the purpose of, for the reason that', and denoting price asked of or paid for goods. It is formed by adding the ending suffix -ért to the end of the noun, e.g. kenyér "bread" >kenyérért "for bread", e.g. elküldtem a boltba kenyérért "I sent him to the store for bread". It is not affected by vowel harmony in Hungarian.Literature
Shibatani
Shibatani lists three criteria for entities and relations that must be encoded in linguistic expressions of causation:- An agent causing or forcing another participant to perform an action, or to be in a certain condition
- The relation between two events is such that the speaker believes that the occurrence of one event, the ‟caused event,” has been realized at t2, which is after t1, the time of the ‟causing event”
- The relation between causing event and caused event is such that the speaker believes the occurrence of the caused event depends wholly on the occurrence of the causing event—the dependency of the two events here must be to the extent that it allows the speaker a counterfactual inference that the caused event would not have taken place at a particular time if the causing event had not taken place, provided that all else had remained the same.
Comrie
focuses on the typology of the syntax and semantics of causative constructions proper. Crucially, Comrie distinguish between the linguistic encoding of causal relations and other extra-linguistic concerns such as the nature of causation itself and questions of how humans perceive of causal relations. While certainly not irrelevant, these extra-linguistic questions will, for now, be left aside. Comrie usefully characterizes causative events in terms of two microevents perceived of composing a macroevent, and encoded in a single expression lexical causatives, in which the two events are expressed in a single lexical item, as in the well-discussed case of English kill; 2) morphological causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in a single verbal complex via causative morphology, and, prototypically, morphological marking showing the status of affected arguments. Finally, Comrie discusses analytic causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in separate clauses.Comrie's work is also noteworthy for having brought the notion of syntactic hierarchy to bear on the typology of causative constructions. A hierarchy of grammatical relations had already been formulated to help explain possibilities for relative clause formation, and Comrie argued that a similar hierarchy was in play, at least in some constructions, in the marking of the original A argument when a base transitive clause is causativized. The hierarchy is as follows:
- subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique > genitive
Song
Presenting a typology of causatives and causation based on a database of 600 languages, Song is very critical of typological work that depends on statistical inference, citing data from the Niger-Congo family that contradicts some earlier claims that “languages within genera are generally fairly similar typologically”. Song therefore culls data from every language for which adequate documentation is available to him, and categorizes the various causative constructions gleaned therefrom into three classes: COMPACT, AND and PURP.Song employs the following terminology:
- – the clause which denotes a causing event
- – the clause which denotes the caused event
- – verbal elements of
- - verbal elements of
The AND causative, for Song, is any construction with a separate and i.e., in which “two clauses involved”. This, in theory, could include larger, multi-clausal expressions of causal relations which many analysts probably would not label a ‘causative construction’, e.g.: ‘It rained yesterday, so they stayed home’, but the boundaries of the AND causative category are not discussed.
One of Song's major contributions to the literature is fleshing out an analysis of his PURP causative. These are constructions which encode intended causation on the part of the causer, but which do not encode any outcome: i.e., the speaker encodes and causer intentionality, but remains agnostic as to whether was felicitously effected.
Talmy
conducts an in-depth investigation of different types of causal relations. Talmy refers to these as “lexicalization patterns,” a term which remains unclear to me, given that few of the examples given in his discussion are lexical items, and most interpretations of “different types of causation incorporated in the verb root” are in fact wholly dependent on other morphosyntactic material in the clause. Let us first examine his list of possible causative types, with examples:- autonomous events The vase broke.
- resulting-event causation The vase broke from a ball’s rolling into it.
- causing-event causation A ball’s rolling into it broke the vase.
- instrument causation A ball broke the vase.
- author causation I broke the vase in rolling a ball into it.
- agent causation I broke the vase by rolling a ball into it.
- undergoer situation My arm broke when I fell.
- self-agentive causation I walked to the store.
- caused agency I sent him to the store.
Indo-European languages
Germanic languages
Proto-Germanic
In Proto-Germanic, the parent language of Germanic languages such as English, causative verbs are formed by adding a suffix -j/ij- to the past-tense ablaut of a strong verb, with Verner's Law voicing applied. Here are some examples:- *rīsaną "to rise" → *raizijaną "to raise", i.e. "to cause to rise"
- *frawerþaną "to perish" → *frawardijaną "to destroy", i.e. "to cause to perish"
- *nesaną "to survive" → *nazjaną "to save", i.e. "to cause to survive"
- *ligjaną "to lie down" → *lagjaną "to lay": "to cause to lie down"
- *grētaną "to weep" → *grōtijaną "to cause to weep"
sitzen/setzen form pairs of causatives/resultatives.
English
English uses various causative mechanisms, with varying degrees of productivity. There are a large number of lexical causatives, such as kill, open and feed.Additionally, there are several morphemes that can express causation. For example, -fy can be thought of as a causative in that it is a derivation that turns an adjective or noun into a "verb of becoming":
- simple → simplify = "to make simple", "to cause to become simple"
- object → objectify = "to make into an object", "to cause to become an object"
As far as lexical causatives are concerned, English has at least 49 causative verbs. Roughly half affect only sentient beings: allow, block, cause, enable, force, get, help, hinder, hold, impede, keep, leave, let, make, permit, prevent, protect, restrain, save, set, start, stimulate, stop. The others can affect either sentient or non-sentient beings: aid, bar, bribe, compel, constrain, convince, deter, discourage, dissuade, drive, have, hamper, impel, incite, induce, influence, inspire, lead, move, persuade, prompt, push, restrict, rouse, send, spur.
Sanskrit
In Sanskrit, there is a causative form of the verb, which is used when the subject of a clause forces or makes the object perform an action. The causative suffix -ay is attached to the verbal root, which may cause vowel sandhi to take place:- bhū "to be, exist" → bhāv-ay; for example, bhāvayati "he causes to be"
- khad "to eat" → khād-ay; for example, khādayāmi "I cause to eat" = "I feed"
Persian
- xordan → xor → xorândan
- xandidan → xand → xandândan
Lithuanian
- skraidyti → skraidinti
- sėdėti → sodinti
- juoktis → juokinti
Latin
Hindi and Urdu
and Urdu use the infix "-ā-" and -vā-" to make verbs causative.- karnā "to do" → karānā "to have done" → "karvānā" → "to have someone make someone do."
- paṛhnā "to read" → parhānā "to make someone read" → "paṛhvānā" "to cause someone to make someone read."
- hilnā "to move" → hilānā "to have something moved" → hilvānā "to have someone make something move."
- pīnā "to drink" → pilānā "to have someone drink" → pilvānā "to have someone make someone drink": "Usne naukrānī se bachchõ-ko pānī pilvāyā" - "She had the maid make the kids drink water."
Bengali
- dêkhā দেখা "to see" → dêkhāno দেখানো "to show/to cause someone to see."
- khāoā খাওয়া "to eat" → khāoāno খাওয়ানো "to feed/to cause someone to eat."
- dêkh দেখ্ → dêkhā দেখা
- khā খা → khāoā খাওয়া
Turkish
In addition to very productive morphological causatives, Turkish also has some lexical causatives: kır- "break", yırt- "split", dik- "plant", yak- "burn", sakla- "hide", aç- "open".Semitic languages
In most Semitic languages, there is a causative form of the verb. It is postulated that in Proto-Semitic, the causative verbal stem was formed by the š- prefix, which has become ʾa-, hi- or ī- in different languages.- Syriac: kəθav "he wrote" → ʾaxtev "he composed"
- Arabic: ʿalima "he knew" → ʾaʿlama "he informed"
- Hebrew: ṣaħak "he laughed" → hiṣħik "he made someone laugh"
- ʿalima "he knew" → ʿallama "he taught"
Japanese
has a morphological device to signify causation. Many verbs undergo internal change in various semiregular patterns between intransitive forms and causative transitive forms:- agaru "to go up, to rise" → ageru "to raise"
- magaru "to turn" → mageru "to bend"
Khmer
has six prefixes and one infix to derive the causative form of verbs, but they vary in frequency and productiveness. The consonantal prefix p- is one of them:- coap "joined" → pcoap "to join"
- cum "around" → pcum "to gather"
Uralic languages
Finnish
Causative forms are also found in the Uralic languages of Europe, such as Finnish:- syödä "to eat" → syöttää "to feed"
- täysi "full" → täyttää "to fill"
- haihtua "to evaporate" → haihduttaa "to vaporize"
- olla "to be" → olettaa "to assume", not "to make exist"
- kirja- ancient "patterns " but modern "book" → kirjoittaa "to write", not "to transform into books"
Hungarian
Austronesian languages
Maori
In Maori, an Austronesian language, the whaka- prefix can be added to a verb:- ako "to learn" becomes whakaako "to teach"
Philippine languages
- dakkel "big " → padakkelen "to enlarge"
- kain "eat" → pakainin "to make eat, to feed"
Guaraní
The -uka suffix is added to transitive verbs:
- ajapo "I make" → japouka "I make do".
- puka "to laugh" → mbopuka "to make laugh"
- guata "to walk" → mboguata "to guide"
- pu'ã "to go up" → mopu'ã "to elevate"
- guata "to walk" → roguata "to make take a walk with "
Uto-Aztecan languages
Classical Nahuatl
, in the Uto-Aztecan language family, has a well-developed morphological system of expressing causation by means of the suffix -tia:- tlacua "he eats" → quitlacualtia "he feeds him/her/it" the causative makes the intransitive verb "eat something" into the bitransitive verb "feed someone something," requiring a pronominal prefix, in this case qui- "him/her/it")
Athabaskan languages
Rice makes the following points about morphological causatives in Athabaskan languages:- In all Athabaskan languages surveyed , the causativizing morphology can causativize at least some intransitive verbs with patientive subjects.
- For intransitive verbs with agentive patients, the family shows a split: only some languages then allow morphological causativization.
- Koyukon was found to be the only language in the survey allowing productive morphological causativization of transitive verbs.
- Perhaps the presence of the direct object pronoun in the causative construction has something to do with whether the causee is human or animate or is capable of being regarded as such. When the causee or the verb cannot be or is not perceived as a potential controller, the pronoun is not found .
Hupa
Golla, in his descriptive grammar of Hupa, describes three classes of morphologically-derived causatives:While Golla does not generalize about the semantics of verb themes that are compatible with causative ƚ-, several preliminary generalizations can be made. Firstly, in the three cases described by Golla, O is neither controlling nor agentive; O is largely patientive in all cases. Secondly, the causer appears to be acting directly on O. Thirdly, none of the examples given involve the causativization of a base-transitive theme.
Central Alaskan Yup'ik
Mithun lists nine causatives for Central Alaskan Yup'uk and describes each in detail. Here is a brief description of each:Morpheme | Approximate meaning |
-vkar-/-cete- | 'let, allow, permit, cause, compel' |
-te- | 'let, allow, cause, compel' |
-nar- | 'cause' |
-rqe- | 'intentionally or deliberately cause' |
-cetaar- | 'try to cause' |
-narqe- | 'tend to cause' |
-naite- | 'tend not to cause' |
-cir- | 'let, wait for, make' |
-i - | 'become or cause to become' |
Bantu-languages
Kinyarwanda
uses periphrastic causatives and morphological causatives.The periphrastic causatives use the verbs -teer- and -tum-, which mean cause. With -teer-, the original S becomes the O of the main clause, leaving the original verb in the infinitive, just like in English:
With -túm-, the original S remains in the embedded clause and the original verb is still marked for person and tense:
Derivational causatives use the -iish- morpheme, which can be applied to intransitives or transitives :
The suffix -iish- implies an indirect causation, but other causatives imply a direct causation.
One of the more direct causation devices is the deletion of what is called a "neutral" morpheme -ik-, which indicates state or potentiality. Stems with the -ik- removed can take -iish, but the causation is then less direct:
Another direct causation maker is -y- which is used for some verbs:
Esperanto
In Esperanto, the suffix -ig- can be added to any kind of word:- morti "to die" → mortigi "to kill"
- pura "clean " → purigi "to clean"
General reading
- Croft, W. 2003. Typology and Universals, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald. 2000. “Introduction”. In Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity, Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds: 1–28. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Goertz, G. et al. 2006. “Use of causatives in Navajo: Syntax and morphology.” In Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics, Volume 18: Proceedings from the Ninth Workshop on American Indigenous Languages.
- Huang, S. and Lily I-Wen Su. 2005. “Iconicity as Evidenced in Saisiyat Linguistic Coding of Causative Events.” Oceanic Linguistics, Vol. 44, No. 2 : 341–356.
- Song, J.J. Linguistic Typology: Morphology and Syntax. Harlow and London: Pearson.
- Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 1: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Causatives of a specific language
- Kinyarwanda: Kimenyi, Alexandre. A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda. University of California Press. p. 160–72.