Morphosyntactic alignment
In linguistics, morphosyntactic alignment is the grammatical relationship between arguments—specifically, between the two arguments of transitive verbs like the dog chased the cat, and the single argument of intransitive verbs like the cat ran away. English has a subject, which merges the more active argument of transitive verbs with the argument of intransitive verbs, leaving the object distinct; other languages may have different strategies, or, rarely, make no distinction at all. Distinctions may be made morphologically, syntactically, or both.
Terminology
Arguments
Dixon (1994)
The following notations will be used to discuss the various types of alignment:- S, the subject of an intransitive verb ;
- A, the subject of a transitive verb;
- O, the object of a transitive verb. Some authors use the label P for O.
In a nominative–accusative system, S and A are grouped together, contrasting O. In an ergative–absolutive system, S and O are one group and contrast with A. The English language represents a typical nominative–accusative system. The name derived from the nominative and accusative cases. Basque is an ergative–absolutive system. The name stemmed from the ergative and absolutive cases. S is said to align with either A or O when they take the same form.
Bickel & Nichols (2009)
Listed below are argument roles used by Bickel and Nichols for the description of alignment types. Their taxonomy is based on semantic roles and valency.- S, the sole argument of a one-place predicate
- A, the more agent-like arguments of a two-place or three-place predicates
- O, the less agent-like argument of a two-place predicate
- G, the more goal-like argument of a three-place predicate
- T, the non-goal-like and non-agent-like argument of a three-place predicate
Locus of marking
Types of alignment
- Nominative–accusative alignment treats the S argument of an intransitive verb like the A argument of transitive verbs, with the O argument distinct . In a language with morphological case marking, an S and an A may both be unmarked or marked with the nominative case while the O is marked with an accusative case, as occurs with nominative -us and accusative -um in Latin: Julius venit "Julius came"; Julius Brutum vidit "Julius saw Brutus". Languages with nominative–accusative alignment can detransitivize transitive verbs by demoting the A argument and promoting the O to be an S ; it is called the passive voice. Most of the world's languages have accusative alignment.
- Ergative–absolutive alignment treats an intransitive argument like a transitive O argument . An A may be marked with an ergative case while the S argument of an intransitive verb and the O argument of a transitive verb are left unmarked or sometimes marked with an absolutive case. Ergative–absolutive languages can detransitivize transitive verbs by demoting the O and promoting the A to an S, thus taking the absolutive case, called the antipassive voice. About a sixth of the world's languages have ergative alignment. The best known are probably Inuit and Basque.
- Active–stative alignment treats the arguments of intransitive verbs like the A argument of transitives in some cases and like transitive O arguments in other cases. For example, in Georgian, Mariamma imğera "Mary sang", Mariam shares the same narrative case ending as in the transitive clause Mariamma c'erili dac'era "Mary wrote the letter ", while in Mariami iq'o Tbilisši revolutsiamde "Mary was in Tbilisi up to the revolution", Mariam shares the same case ending as the object of the transitive clause. Thus, the arguments of intransitive verbs are not uniform in its behaviour.
- Austronesian alignment, also called Philippine-type alignment, is found in the Austronesian languages of the Philippines, Borneo, Taiwan, and Madagascar. These languages have both accusative-type and ergative-type alignments in transitive verbs. They are traditionally called "active" and "passive" voice because the speaker can choose to use either one rather like active and passive voice in English. However, because they are not true voice, terms such as "agent trigger" or "actor focus" are increasingly used for the accusative type and "patient trigger" or "undergoer focus" for the ergative type.. Patient-trigger alignment is the default in most of these languages. For either alignment, two core cases are used, but the same morphology is used for the "nominative" of the agent-trigger alignment and the "absolutive" of the patient-trigger alignment so there is a total of just three core cases: common S/A/O, ergative A, and accusative O. Some Austronesianists argue that these languages have four alignments, with additional "voices" that mark a locative or benefactive with the direct case, but most maintain that these are not core arguments and thus not basic to the system.
- Direct alignment: a very few languages make no distinction among agent, patient, and intransitive arguments, leaving the hearer to rely entirely on context and common sense to figure them out. This S/A/O case is called direct, as it sometimes is in Austronesian alignment.
- Tripartite alignment uses a separate case or syntax for each argument, which are conventionally called the accusative case, the intransitive case, and the ergative case. The Nez Perce language is a notable example.
- Transitive alignment: certain Iranian languages, such as Rushani, distinguish only transitivity, using a transitive case for both A and O, and an intransitive case for S. That is sometimes called a double-oblique system, as the transitive case is equivalent to the accusative in the non-past tense.
In addition, in some languages, both nominative–accusative and ergative–absolutive systems may be used, split between different grammatical contexts, called split ergativity. The split may sometimes be linked to animacy, as in many Australian Aboriginal languages, or to aspect, as in Hindustani and Mayan languages. A few Australian languages, such as Diyari, are split among accusative, ergative, and tripartite alignment, depending on animacy.
A popular idea, introduced in Anderson, is that some constructions universally favor accusative alignment while others are more flexible. In general, behavioral constructions are claimed to favor nominative–accusative alignment while coding constructions do not show any alignment preferences. This idea underlies early notions of ‘deep’ vs. ‘surface’ ergativity : many languages have surface ergativity only but not in their behavioral constructions or at least not in all of them. Languages with deep ergativity appear to be less common.
Comparison between Ergative–Absolutive and Nominative–Accusative
The arguments can be symbolized as follows:- O = most patient-like argument of a transitive clause
- S = sole argument of an intransitive clause
- A = most agent-like argument of a transitive clause
The relationship between ergative and accusative systems can be schematically represented as the following:
Ergative–absolutive | Nominative–accusative | |
O | same | different |
S | same | same |
A | different | same |
The following Basque examples demonstrate ergative–absolutive case marking system:
In Basque, gizona is "the man" and mutila is "the boy". In a sentence like mutila gizonak ikusi du, you know who is seeing whom because -k is added to the one doing the seeing. So the sentence means "the man saw the boy". If you want to say "the boy saw the man", add the -k instead to the word meaning "the boy": mutilak gizona ikusi du.
With a verb like etorri, "come", there's no need to distinguish "who is doing the coming", so no -k is added. "The boy came" is mutila etorri da.
Japanese – by contrast – marks nouns by following them with different particles which indicate their function in the sentence:
In this language, in the sentence "the man saw the child", the one doing the seeing may be marked with ga, which works like Basque -k. However, in sentences like "the child arrived" ga can still be used even though the situation involves only a "doer" and not a "done-to". This is unlike Basque, where -k is completely forbidden in such sentences.