Syntax
In linguistics, syntax is the set of rules, principles, and processes that govern the structure of sentences in a given language, usually including word order. The term syntax is also used to refer to the study of such principles and processes. The goal of many syntacticians is to discover the syntactic rules common to all languages.
Etymology
The word syntax comes from Ancient Greek: "coordination", which consists of σύν syn, "together", and τάξις táxis, "an ordering".Sequencing of subject, verb, and object
One basic description of a language's syntax is the sequence in which the subject, verb, and object usually appear in sentences. Over 85% of languages usually place the subject first, either in the sequence SVO or the sequence SOV. The other possible sequences are VSO, VOS, OVS, and OSV, the last three of which are rare. In most generative theories of syntax, these surface differences arise from a more complex clausal phrase structure, and each order may be compatible with multiple derivations.Early history
The Aṣṭādhyāyī of Pāṇini, is often cited as an example of a premodern work that approaches the sophistication of a modern syntactic theory. In the West, the school of thought that came to be known as "traditional grammar" began with the work of Dionysius Thrax.For centuries, a framework known as grammaire générale dominated work in syntax: as its basic premise the assumption that language is a direct reflection of thought processes and therefore there is a single, most natural way to express a thought.
However, in the 19th century, with the development of historical-comparative linguistics, linguists began to realize the sheer diversity of human language and to question fundamental assumptions about the relationship between language and logic. It became apparent that there was no such thing as the most natural way to express a thought, and therefore logic could no longer be relied upon as a basis for studying the structure of language.
The Port-Royal grammar modeled the study of syntax upon that of logic. Syntactic categories were identified with logical ones, and all sentences were analyzed in terms of "subject – copula – predicate". Initially, this view was adopted even by the early comparative linguists such as Franz Bopp.
The central role of syntax within theoretical linguistics became clear only in the 20th century, which could reasonably be called the "century of syntactic theory" as far as linguistics is concerned.
Theories
There are a number of theoretical approaches to the discipline of syntax. One school of thought, founded in the works of Derek Bickerton, sees syntax as a branch of biology, since it conceives of syntax as the study of linguistic knowledge as embodied in the human mind. Other linguists take a more Platonistic view, since they regard syntax to be the study of an abstract formal system. Yet others consider syntax a taxonomical device to reach broad generalizations across languages.Dependency grammar
is an approach to sentence structure where syntactic units are arranged according to the dependency relation, as opposed to the constituency relation of phrase structure grammars. Dependencies are directed links between words. The verb is seen as the root of all clause structure and all the other words in the clause are either directly or indirectly dependent on this root. Some prominent dependency-based theories of syntax are:- Recursive categorical syntax, or Algebraic syntax
- Functional generative description
- Meaning–text theory
- Operator grammar
- Word grammar
Categorial grammar
is an approach that attributes the syntactic structure not to rules of grammar, but to the properties of the syntactic categories themselves. For example, rather than asserting that sentences are constructed by a rule that combines a noun phrase and a verb phrase , in categorial grammar, such principles are embedded in the category of the head word itself. So the syntactic category for an intransitive verb is a complex formula representing the fact that the verb acts as a function word requiring an NP as an input and produces a sentence level structure as an output. This complex category is notated as instead of V. NP\S is read as "a category that searches to the left for an NP and outputs a sentence." The category of transitive verb is defined as an element that requires two NPs to form a sentence. This is notated as which means "a category that searches to the right for an NP, and generates a function which is, which in turn represents a function that searches to the left for an NP and produces a sentence."Tree-adjoining grammar is a categorial grammar that adds in partial tree structures to the categories.
Stochastic/probabilistic grammars/network theories
Theoretical approaches to syntax that are based upon probability theory are known as stochastic grammars. One common implementation of such an approach makes use of a neural network or connectionism.Functional grammars
Functionalist models of grammar study the form–function interaction by performing a structural and a functional analysis.- Functional discourse grammar
- Prague linguistic circle
- Role and reference grammar
- Systemic functional grammar
Generative grammar
Among the many generative theories of linguistics, the Chomskyan theories are:
Other theories that find their origin in the generative paradigm are:
- Arc pair grammar
- Generalized phrase structure grammar
- Generative semantics
- Head-driven phrase structure grammar
- Lexical functional grammar
- Nanosyntax
- Relational grammar
- Harmonic grammar
Cognitive and usage-based grammars
- Cognitive grammar
- Construction grammar
- Emergent grammar
Syntactic terms
- Adjective
- Adjective phrase
- Adjunct
- Adpositional phrase
- Adverb
- Anaphora
- Answer ellipsis
- Antecedent
- Antecedent-contained deletion
- Appositive
- Argument
- Article
- Aspect
- Attributive adjective and predicative adjective
- Auxiliary verb
- Binding
- Branching
- c-command
- Case
- Category
- Catena
- Clause
- Closed class word
- Comparative
- Complement
- Compound noun and adjective
- Conjugation
- Conjunction
- Constituent
- Coordination
- Coreference
- Crossover
- Dangling modifier
- Declension
- Dependency grammar
- Dependent marking
- Determiner
- Discontinuity
- Do-support
- Dual
- Ellipsis
- Endocentric
- Exceptional case-marking
- Expletive
- Extraposition
- Finite verb
- Function word
- Gapping
- Gender
- Gerund
- Government
- Head
- Head marking
- Infinitive
- Inverse copular construction
- Inversion
- Lexical item
- m-command
- Measure word
- Merge
- Modal particle
- Modal verb
- Modifier
- Mood
- Movement
- Movement paradox
- Nanosyntax
- Negative inversion
- Non-configurational language
- Non-finite verb
- Noun
- Noun ellipsis
- Noun phrase
- Number
- Object
- Open class word
- Parasitic gap
- Part of speech
- Particle
- Periphrasis
- Person
- Personal pronoun
- Pied-piping
- Phrasal verb
- Phrase
- Phrase structure grammar
- Plural
- Predicate
- Predicative expression
- Preposition and postposition
- Pronoun
- Pseudogapping
- Raising
- Relation
- Restrictiveness
- Right node raising
- Sandhi
- Scrambling
- Selection
- Sentence
- Separable verb
- Shifting
- Singular
- Sluicing
- Small clause
- Stripping
- Subcategorization
- Subject
- Subject-auxiliary inversion
- Subject-verb inversion
- Subordination
- Superlative
- Tense
- Topicalization
- Tough movement
- Uninflected word
- V2 word order
- Valency
- Verb
- Verb phrase
- Verb phrase ellipsis
- Voice
- Wh-movement
- Word order
- X-bar theory