Consonant harmony


Consonant harmony is a type of "long-distance" phonological assimilation, akin to the similar assimilatory process involving vowels, i.e. vowel harmony.
A good discussion of consonant harmony typology is found in Rose and Walker's 2004 paper in the journal Language, "A Typology of Consonant Agreement as Correspondence".

Examples

In Athabaskan languages

One of the more common harmony processes is coronal harmony, which affects coronal fricatives, such as s and sh. Then, all coronal fricatives belong to the +anterior class or the -anterior class. Such patterns are found in the Dene languages such as Navajo, Tahltan, Western Apache, and in Chumash on the California coast. In Tahltan, Shaw showed that coronal harmony affects three coronal fricatives, s, sh and the interdental th. The following examples are given by de Reuse: in Western Apache, the verbal prefix si- is an alveolar fricative, as in the following forms:
However, when the prefix si- occurs before a verb stem that contains a post-alveolar affricate, the si- surfaces as the post-alveolar shi-:
Thus, all sibilant obstruents in these languages are divided into two groups, +anterior and -anterior. In Navajo, as in most languages with consonant harmony, there is a constraint on the shape of roots that is identical to the harmony process. All roots with sibiliant affricates or fricatives have the same value for anteriority. Shaw provides a phonological analysis of this process, using data from research on Tahltan.
There are two interesting aspects of the process in Navajo. Firstly, morphemes that participate are domain-specific, only the last two domains are affected. Verbal morphemes from the outer or 'disjunct' domain are not affected by the process: the process is morphologically conditioned. Secondly, the lateral affricate and fricative appear with both values. Young and Morgan offer an extensive sets of examples of this type of morpheme alternation in Navajo.

In Sanskrit

A different example of coronal harmony, sometimes referred to as NATI rule, occurs in Sanskrit, where is retroflexed to if it is preceded by a retroflex continuant, mainly and, in the same word, even at a distance. The retroflexion spreads from left to right affecting any coronal nasal until the word boundary is reached. This phenomenon, however, is blocked whenever a coronal plosive is placed between / and. For instance, in the noun ब्राह्मण brāhmaṇa "Brahmin priest", the original coronal of the action noun suffix -man changes to a retroflex due to consonant harmony triggered by . On the other hand, in the word अर्चन arcana "homage, praising" consonant harmony is prevented by the coronal stop which blocks the assimilation.

In Old Chinese

probably had some constraint governing the shape of disyllables. According to modern reconstructions of Old Chinese phonology, type A and B syllables almost never co-occur in a disyllabic word. In the latest reconstruction of Old Chinese phonology proposed by Baxter and Sagart, this type A vs. type B distinction can be traced back to the presence or the absence of pharyngealization respectively, cf. nà < OC "to bring into" and rù < OC "to enter" only differing by the trait of the initial consonant. Onsets of type B syllables, lacking of pharyngealization, are subject to palatalization in Middle Chinese, while type A pharyngealized onsets failed to palatalize. In many ancient disyllabic words type A and type B characters do not mix, there are almost solely bisyllabic morphemes either with type A syllables, such as:
or with type B syllables, including:
Such pattern seems to suggest the existence of some sort of pharyngeal harmony in Old Chinese. However, there are notable, though infrequent, exceptions to this tendency, manifesting in ancient compounds that are generally hard to analyze. From this list the following word is often mentioned:
Various Austronesian languages have consonant harmony among the liquid consonants, with assimilating at a distance to or vice versa.
Guaraní shows nasal harmony, and certain affixes have alternative forms according to whether the root includes a nasal or not. For example, the reflexive prefix is realized as oral je- before an oral stem like juka "kill", but as nasal ñe- before a nasal stem like nupã "hit". The ã makes the stem nasal.
Some Finnish-speakers find it hard to pronounce both 'b' and 'p' in loanwords and so they voice or devoice the entire word. It should, however, be noted that the distinction between the consonants is not native to Finnish. Native Finnish words do not use.
In the Ngeté-Herdé language of Cameroon, voicing of word-medial obstruents is strongly influenced by voicing of the word-initial consonant. Generally, all obstruents in a word are either voiced or voiceless.