Proto-Samic
Proto-Sami is the hypothetical, reconstructed common ancestor of the Sami languages. It is a descendant of the Proto-Uralic language.
Homeland and expansion
Although the current Sami languages are spoken much further to the north and west, Proto-Sami was likely spoken in the area of modern-day Southwestern Finland around the first few centuries CE. Local ancestors of the modern Sami people likely still spoke non-Uralic, "Paleoeuropean" languages at this point. This situation can be traced in placenames as well as through the analysis of loanwords from Germanic, Baltic and Finnic. Evidence also can be found for the existence of language varieties closely related to but likely distinct from Sami proper having been spoken further east, with a limit around Lake Beloye.Separation of the main branches is also likely to have occurred in southern Finland, with these later independently spreading north into Sápmi. The exact routes of this are not clear: it is possible Western Sami entered Scandinavia across Kvarken rather than via land. Concurrently, Finnic languages that would eventually end up becoming modern-day Finnish and Karelian were being adopted in the southern end of the Proto-Sami area, likely in connection with the introduction of agriculture, a process that continued until the 19th century, leading to the extirpation of original Sami languages in Karelia and all but northernmost Finland.
Phonology
Consonants
The Proto-Sami consonant inventory is mostly faithfully retained from Proto-Uralic, and is considerably smaller than what is typically found in modern Sami languages. There were 16 contrastive consonants, most of which could however occur both short and geminate:Bilabial | Dental | Alveolar | Palatal | Velar | |
Stops and affricates | | | | | |
Nasals | | | |||
Fricatives | | | |||
Approximants | | | | ||
Trill | |
Stop and affricate consonants were split in three main allophones with respect to phonation:
- Plain voiceless,, etc, occurred word-initially, adjacent to other voiceless consonants, and in the strong grade of single intervocalic consonants
- Lax voiceless,, etc, occurred between voiced sounds
- Preaspirated,, etc, occurred in geminates
Consonant gradation
A detailed system of allophony is reconstructible, known as consonant gradation. Gradation applied to all intervocalic single consonants as well as all consonant clusters. This is unlike gradation in the related Proto-Finnic and its descendants, where it applied only to a subset. The conditioning factor was the same, however: the weak grade occurred if the following syllable was closed, the strong grade if it was open. This difference was originally probably realized as length:- A single consonant was short in the weak grade, e.g., half-long in the strong grade
- A geminate consonant was long in the weak grade, overlong in the strong grade
- A consonant cluster had a short 1st member in the weak grade, e.g., a half-long one in the strong grade,
In sources on Proto-Sami reconstruction, gradation is often assumed but not indicated graphically. In this article, when it is relevant and necessary to show the distinction, the weak grade is denoted with an inverted breve below the consonant: s : s̯, č : č̯, tt : t̯t̯, lk : l̯k̯.
After the phonematization of gradation due to loss of word-final sounds, Sami varieties could be left with as many as four different contrastive degrees of consonant length. This has only been attested in some dialects of Ume Sami. Most other Sami varieties phonemically merged the weak grade of geminates with the strong grade of single consonants, leaving only three lengths. In some Sami languages, other sound developments have left only two or three degrees occurring elsewhere.
Vowels
An asymmetric system of four short and five long vowel segments can be reconstructed.- The four diphthongs only occurred in stressed syllables, in complementary distribution with the two long vowels occurring in unstressed syllables.
- did not generally occur in the last syllable of a word.
Prosody
- P
- P _
- P _ _
- P _ S _
- P _ S _ _
- P _ S _ S _
- etc.
This alternation survives in many Sami languages in the form of distinct inflectional classes, with words with a stressed second-last syllable following the so-called "even" or "two-syllable" inflection, and words with an unstressed second-last syllable following the "odd" or "three-syllable" inflection. Weakening and simplification of non-final consonants after unstressed syllables contributed further to the alternation, leading to differences that are sometimes quite striking. For example:
In compounds, which consisted of a combination of several root words, each word retained the stress pattern that it had in isolation, so that that stress remained lexically significant. The first syllable of the first part of a compound had the strongest stress, with progressively weaker secondary stress for the first syllables of the remaining parts.
Grammar
Nominals
s, i.e. nouns, adjectives, numerals and pronouns were systematically inflected for two numbers and ten cases. The personal pronouns and possessive suffixes also distinguished the dual number.Cases
The cases included the core cases nominative, accusative and genitive; the local cases inessive, elative, illative; as well as essive, partitive, comitative and abessive.Case | Singular ending | Plural ending | Meaning/use |
Nominative | ∅ | *-k | Subject, object of imperative |
Accusative | *-m | *-jtē | Object |
Partitive | *-tē | *-jtē | Partial object, motion away |
Genitive | *-n | *-j | Possession, relation |
Essive | *-nē | *-jnē | Being, acting as |
Inessive | *-snē | *-jnē | Being at, on, inside |
Elative | *-stē | *-jstē | Motion from, off, out of |
Illative | *-s̯ën | *-jtēs̯ën *-jtē *-j̯t̯ën | Motion towards, to, onto, into |
Comitative | *-jnē *-jnë | *-j | With, in company of, by means of |
Abessive | *-ptāk̯ëk | - | Without, lacking |
Several of the singular cases do not have a clear counterpart in the plural, or have different formations ancestral to different Sami languages. For example, what would later become the accusative plural developed out of the partitive plural form, while the inessive plural is the original essive plural form. The comitative plural was in origin a periphrastic construction consisting of the genitive plural with the noun *kuojmē "companion". It is likely that the case system was still partially in development during the late Proto-Sami period, and developed in subtly different ways in the various descendants.
In most Sami languages, the case system has been simplified:
- The partitive has been lost in most western languages.
- In several languages the genitive and accusative singular have coincided, and in Northern Sami this led to an analogical merger in the plural. Southern and Pite Sami still keep the two cases separate.
- A sound change *sn > *st that occurred in the history of several Sami languages caused a merging of the inessive and elative singular, creating a single "locative" case. Several languages merged the plural cases analogically, but some languages chose the former inessive plural form, while others chose the elative plural.
Possession
Verb inflection
- The conditional mood had the mood marker *-kćē-, to which past tense endings were attached. In Western Sami, a new conditional mood was innovated, consisting of the connegative form of the verb joined to a past-tense form of the copula *leatēk.
- The potential mood had the mood marker *-ńćë-. It received present-tense endings.
- Infinitive *-tēk, identical with the Finnic ending *-dak.
- Verbal noun *-mē, identical with the Finnic verbal noun suffix *-ma.
- Present participle *-jē, originally an agent noun suffix, cognate to the Finnic agent noun suffix *-ja.
- Past participle *-më or extended *-mëńćë. The extended form is identical with the Finnish verbal noun/"fourth infinitive" suffix *-minen ~ *-mice-.
Lexicon
Development
From Proto-Uralic
- followed by > followed by.
- >, a change shared with the Finnic and Mordvinic languages. This change counterfeeds the previous one.
- >.
- Loss of vowel harmony. In non-initial syllables, front and back harmonic allophones collapsed into one: > and >.
- >, a development also shared with Finnic and Mordvinic.
- >
- Vowels are lengthened before.
- > before a vowel. is lost elsewhere.
- >
- >
- >
Examples:
- PU > preS > PS 'uncle'
- PU > preS > PS 'glue'
- PU > preS > PS 'tree stump'
- PU > preS > PS 'to sell'
- Baltic → preS > PS 'frost'
- Germanic 'red' → preS > PS 'iron'
- Germanic → preS > PS 'guest'
- Finnic → preS > PS 'rear'
- substrate? → preS > PS 'rock', in place of Uralic
- substrate? → preS or > PS "wood", in place of Uralic or
- substrate? → preS > PS 'perch'
- substrate? → preS > PS 'feather'
- Geminate fricatives were introduced in certain loanwords.
- was denasalized before a heterorganic obstruent.
- * PU → PS 'bow'
- * PU → PS 'cavity'
Vowel shift
The previous changes left a system consisting of in the first syllable in Pre-Sami, and probably at least long. In unstressed syllables, only were distinguished. The source of is unclear, although it is frequently also found in Finnic.
The table below shows the main correspondences:
The processes that added up to this shift can be outlined as follows:
Further changes then shifted the sound values of the unstressed syllables that had conditioned the above shift:
The effects of the vowel shift can be illustrated by the following comparison between Northern Sami, and Finnish, known for retaining vowel values very close to Proto-Uralic. All word pairs correspond to each other regularly:
Towards the modern Sami languagesThe main division among the Sami languages is the split between eastern and western Sami.Changes that appear across the Eastern-Western divide are:
UmlautIn the history of Proto-Sami, some sound changes were triggered or prevented by the nature of the vowel in the next syllable. Such changes continued to occur in the modern Sami languages, but differently in each. Due to the similarity with Germanic umlaut, these phenomena are termed "umlaut" as well.The following gives a comparative overview of each possible Proto-Sami vowel in the first syllable, with the outcomes that are found in each language for each second-syllable vowel. Long openLong open-mid
Short close
|