Continuous and progressive aspects


The continuous and progressive aspects are grammatical aspects that express incomplete action or state in progress at a specific time: they are non-habitual, imperfective aspects.
In the grammars of many languages the two terms are used interchangeably. This is also the case with English: a construction such as "He is washing" may be described either as present continuous or as present progressive. However, there are certain languages for which two different aspects are distinguished. In Chinese, for example, progressive aspect denotes a current action, as in "he is getting dressed", while continuous aspect denotes a current state, as in "he is wearing fine clothes".
As with other grammatical categories, the precise semantics of the aspects vary from language to language, and from grammarian to grammarian. For example, some grammars of Turkish count the -iyor form as a present tense; some as a progressive tense; and some as both a continuous and a progressive aspect.

Continuous versus progressive

The progressive aspect expresses the dynamic quality of actions that are in progress while the continuous aspect expresses the state of the subject that is continuing the action. For instance, "Tom is reading" can express dynamic activity: "Tom is reading a book" - i.e. right now, or Tom's current state: "Tom is reading for a degree" - i.e. Tom is a student. The aspect can often be ambiguous; "Tom is reading Ulysses" may describe his current activity, or the state of having started, but not yet finished, the book.

Continuous and progressive in various languages

Unless otherwise indicated, the following languages treat continuous and progressive aspects the same, in which case the term continuous is used to refer to both.

English

Use

The continuous aspect is constructed by using a form of the copula, "to be", together with the present participle. It is generally used for actions that are occurring at the time in question, and does not focus on the larger time-scale. For example, the sentence "Andrew was playing tennis when Jane called him." indicates what Andrew was doing when Jane called him, but does not indicate for how long Andrew played, nor how often he plays; for that, the simple past would suffice: "Andrew played tennis three hours every day for several years."
Salikoko Mufwene contrasts the effect of the progressive form on the meanings of action verbs versus those of lexically stative verbs:

Origin

The progressive aspect in English likely arose from two constructions that were used fairly rarely in Old and Early Middle English. The first used a form of beon/wesan with a present participle. This construction has an analogous form in Dutch. The second used beon/wesan, a preposition, and a gerund, and has been variously proposed as being influenced by similar forms in Latin and French or British Celtic, though evidence one way or another is scant. Over the course of the Middle English period, sound shifts in the language meant that the participle ending and the gerund ending merged into a new ending,. This change, which was complete in southern England around the late fifteenth century and spread north from there, rendered participles and gerunds indistinguishable. It is at this point that a sudden increase in the use of progressive forms is visible, though they would not take their current form until the eighteenth century. Linguist Herbert Schendl has concluded that "with this feature, a polygenetic origin... seems attractive, and at least the further extension of the progressive is a language-internal development."

Berber

In the Amazigh language, past continuous is formed by using the fixed participle ttugha ; ttugha is added before the verb that is in the present tense. So we have:
Ntta itari : he writes / he is writing
Ntta ttugha itari : he was writing
Present continuous is usually the same as the present tense. But in the Riff variety of Berber, the participle aqqa is added before the verb to form present continuous.

Chinese

is one family of languages that makes a distinction between the continuous and progressive aspects.

Cantonese

has a very regular system for expressing aspects via verb suffixes. is typically used to express progressive aspect while is used to express continuous aspect. Take the following example:
English Translation
Progressive ' I am putting on clothes.
Continuous ' I am wearing clothes.

In the example, the progressive aspect expresses the fact that the subject is actively putting on clothes rather than merely wearing them as in the continuous aspect. This example is useful for showing English speakers the difference between continuous and progressive because "wearing" in English never conveys the progressive aspect..
In Cantonese, the progressive marker 緊 can express the continuous aspect as well, depending on the context, but in general, the progressive aspect is assumed. In order to emphasize the progressive aspect rather than the continuous, can be used in front of the verb:
' '
喺度 can also be used without 緊 to indicate the progressive aspect.

Mandarin

Unlike Cantonese, Mandarin does not have a verb suffix for expressing the progressive aspect, but it can use the pre-verbal auxiliary zhèngzài , similar to how Cantonese uses 喺度 in front of the verb. The continuous aspect does have a verb suffix, zhe, which is cognate with the Cantonese label=none in this context.
English Translation
Progressive ' I am putting on clothes.
Continuous ' I am wearing clothes.

For more information see.

Danish

Danish has several ways of constructing continuous aspect.
The continuous aspect is commonly used in Dutch, though not as often as in English. There are various methods of forming a continuous:
does not have a continuous aspect per se; events that English would describe using its continuous aspect, French would describe using a neutral aspect. Many express what they are doing in French by just using the present tense. That being said, French can express a continuous sense using the periphrastic construction être en train de ; for example, English's "we were eating" might be expressed in French either as nous étions en train de manger, or as simply nous mangions.
An exception is in relating events that took place in the past: the imperfect has a continuous aspect in relation to the simple past; e.g. nous mangions quand il frappa à la porte. However, the passé composé is more often used to denote past events with a neutral aspect in a non-narrative context.
It is also possible to use the present participle, e.g. Nous mangeant, il frappait à la porte, or the gerund.
Quebec French often expresses a continuous sense using the periphrastic construction être après ; for example, English's "we were eating" might be expressed in Quebec French either as nous étions après manger, or as simply nous mangions.

Jèrriais

Formed exactly as in Rhinish German, Jèrriais constructs the continuous with verb êt' + à + infinitive. For example, j'têmes à mangi translates as we were eating.

German

There is no continuous aspect in standard German. The aspect can be expressed with gerade as in er liest gerade meaning he is reading. Certain regional dialects, such as those of the Rhineland, the Ruhr Area, and Westphalia, form a continuous aspect using the verb sein, the inflected preposition am or beim, and the neuter noun that is formed from an infinitive. This construction was likely borrowed from Low German or Dutch which use the exact construction to convey the same meaning. For example, ich bin am Lesen, ich bin beim Lesen means I am reading. Known as the rheinische Verlaufsform, it has become increasingly common in the casual speech of many speakers around Germany through popular media and music, although it is still frowned upon in formal and literary contexts. In Southern Austro-Bavarian, the aspect can be expressed using tun as an auxiliary with the infinitive of the verb as in er tut lesen for he is reading.

Hawaiian

In Hawaiian, the present tense progressive aspect form ke + verb + nei is very frequently used.

Hindi and Urdu

and Urdu have a definite progressive/continuous aspect, marked by auxiliaries, for past, present, and future. It is distinguished from the habitual aspect, and is widely used in everyday speech. Like English, it is also used to denote an immediate future action. Hindustani uses the habitual construction for both the stative aspect and the progressive one. For a complete conjugation of the continuous tenses, see Hindustani grammar.
The continuous/progressive aspect is marked by a progressive participle rahā which agrees with the gender and number of the subject and is followed by the copula honā in proper conjugation.
There are three aspects in Hindi/Urdu, Perfective, Habitual and Progressive. Only the 3rd person conjugations are shown in the table below.
Notes:
  1. The future and the presumptive mood of Hindi have the same forms for the Progressive aspect. When the context is about the future the copula is assumed to be of the future tense and when the context is of the past, copula is assumed to be of the presumptive mood. The exact detail in the context of the past would tell whether to assume the presumptive present or the presumptive past. For example, "do sāl pehle bèṭh rahā hogā" and "abhī bèṭh rahā hogā." both have the same presumptive part as the table above but do sāl pehle means "two years ago" and abhī means "right now" which makes it clear that the first sentence is in presumptive present and the second sentence is in presumptive past.
  2. The verb bèṭhnā in Hindi/Urdu when used with the progressive participle means "the person is in the process to sit" and not is actually already sitting. The translation in the table above talks about the progressive state of sitting from the state of not sitting and not the continued state of sitting. To show the continued state of sitting either the perfective participle progressive form is used or is used. The former translates as "...has/have stayed sitting" and latter as "...is sitting". Such continuous forms are not considered as an aspect in itself in Hindi/Urdu since huā is used only to mark verbal adjectives, it has no part in the aspect/tense/mood system.
  3. The habitual and the perfective participles of Hindi and Urdu can also be used with the progressive participle to form the progressive forms and the progressive habitual forms.
  4. Like many other such for example Finnish, Romanian, and Polish, the conditional form of the verbs in Hindi/Urdu appear twice in a conditional phrase in contrast to many Romance Languages where there are separate forms for the conditional and the past subjunctive. In Hindi/Urdu both the apodosis and the protasis put the verb in the conditional/contrafactual. The past subjunctive and the conditional mood is marked in Hindi/Urdu with the same contrafactual marker.

    Icelandic

possesses a present continuous aspect much like that found in English. This feature is unique among the Scandinavian languages. It is formed with the copula vera + + infinite verb. Its usage differs slightly from English, as it generally cannot be used in static contexts, for example standing or sitting, but rather to describe specific activities. The following examples illustrate this phenomenon.
In contrast with:
In the second example, the simple present tense is used as it describes a state, standing on the table. The construction *ég er að standa á borðinu is incorrect in Icelandic. In addition this method of constructing the continuous present there exists a second method akin to the one which exists in the other Scandinavian languages, where a present participle ending in -andi is used along with the copula vera. This is a way of using the present participle that is analysed as more adjectival or adverbial than verbal, as it cannot be used with transitive verbs. With certain verbs it also has a frequentative implication, as in the following example:
Technically the use of the present participle is often not an example of continuous aspect in Icelandic.

Italian

forms a progressive aspect in much the same way as in Spanish, using a conjugated form of the verb stare followed by the gerund of the main verb. There are only two forms of gerunds, the choice depending upon the ending of the main verb in the infinitive: -ando for verbs whose infinitive ends in -are or -endo if the infinitive ends in -ere or -ire. Thus 'I am speaking/reading/sleeping' is expressed Sto parlando/leggendo/dormendo.

Present tense

The present tense and the present progressive can have distinct meanings in Italian. Both can be used for present-time actions in progress: parlo con Mario and sto parlando con Mario can both mean 'I'm speaking with Mario ', but only the bare present can be used to express ongoing state, as in parlo inglese 'I speak English', i.e. to convey the information 'I am able to speak English'. Thus the present progressive clarifies immediacy: Sto uscendo 'I'm leaving '.
The present continuous is formed by using the present tense of the verb stare + the gerund. As in English, the gerund conveys the main meaning of the utterance: sto pattinando, I am skating. For the regular verbs, the gerund is formed from the infinitive of the verb by taking the stem and attaching the appropriate gerund suffix: -are verbs take -ando and the -ere and -ire verbs both take -endo. The table shows the conjugations of stare in the present tense with a gerund to exemplify the present continuous:
The present continuous tense has a very predictable conjugation pattern even for verbs that are typically irregular, such as essere and avere. For verbs with reduced infinitives, the gerund uses the same stem as the imperfect.

Past tense

To form the past progressive, stare is conjugated in the imperfect and used with the gerund. For example, while sto andando means "I am going", stavo andando expresses I was going. In conventional Italian speaking, stavo andando and imperfect andavo are mostly interchangeable in the progressive meaning, whereas past habitual "I used to go", "I went " is expressible only with the imperfect andavo.
Conjugations of the Past Progressive:
Like the present progressive, the Italian past progressive is extremely regular. Forms of stare are those common to -are verbs in the imperfect.
There is no readily available means in Italian for expressing the distinction between English "We were reading" and "We have been reading."

Japanese

Standard Japanese uses the same grammar form to form the progressive and the continuous aspect, specifically by using the -te iru form of a verb. Depending on the transitivity of the verb, they are interpreted as either progressive or continuous. For example:
Intransitive:
Transitive:
Some dialects such as Chūgoku dialect and Shikoku dialect have different grammar forms for the progressive and the continuous aspect; the -toru form for the progressive and the -yoru form for the continuous. For example:
Continuous:
Progressive:

Portuguese

In Portuguese the continuous aspect is marked by gerund, either by a proper -ndo ending or a and the infinitive ; for example to be doing would be either estar a fazer or, similar to other Romance languages, estar fazendo.

Quechua

uses a specific suffix: -chka or -ykaa; which is directly attached before the conjugation suffixes. Although the continuous aspect in Quechua is similar to that of English, it is more used than the simple tenses and is commonly translated into them, because of the idea that actions are not instantaneous, but they have a specific duration.

Slavic languages

In Slavic languages there is a clear distinction between perfective and imperfective grammatical aspects in the verb stem, with the latter emphasizing that the action is or was in progress. It was in relation to these languages that the modern concept of grammatical aspect in general originally developed. Majority of verbs in Slavic languages have at least one matching pair of the other aspect – e.g. Czech ' and ' which would translate as "to buy" and "be buying" respectively.
Perfective verbs are commonly formed from imperfective ones by the addition of a prefix, or else the imperfective verb is formed from the perfective one by modification of the stem or ending. Suppletion also plays a small role. Perfective verbs cannot generally be used with the meaning of a present tense – their present-tense forms in fact have future reference. An example of such a pair of verbs, from Polish, is given below:
In at least the East Slavic and West Slavic languages, there is a three-way aspect differentiation for verbs of motion, with two forms of imperfective, determinate and indeterminate, and one form of perfective. The two forms of imperfective can be used in all three tenses, but the perfective can only be used with past and future. The indeterminate imperfective expresses habitual aspect, while the determinate imperfective expresses progressive aspect. The difference corresponds closely to that between the English "I go to school" and "I am going to school ". The three-way difference is given below for the Russian basic verbs of motion. When prefixes are attached to Russian verbs of motion, they become more or less normal imperfective/perfective pairs, although the prefixes are generally attached to the indeterminate imperfective to form the prefixed imperfective and to the determinate imperfective to form the prefixed perfective. For example, prefix при- + indeterminate ходи́ть = приходи́ть; and prefix при- + determinate идти́ = прийти.

Spanish

In Spanish, the continuous is constructed much as in English, using a conjugated form of estar plus the gerundio of the main verb; for example, estar haciendo means to be doing.
Like English, Spanish also has a few related constructions with similar structures and related meanings; for example, seguir haciendo means to keep doing.
Conjugations of the Present Progressive in Spanish:

Footnotes